The Psychological Record, 2000, 50, 79-104 PROBABILITY OF STIMULUS EQUIVALENCE AS A FUNCTION OF CLASS SIZE VS. NUMBER OF CLASSES ERIK ARNTZEN and PER HOLTH University of as/a, Norway Two experiments were conducted to study stimulus equivalence as a function of class size and number of classes. In the first experiment, equivalence was tested in 50 normal adult subjects following a linear series training structure. Subjects were successively assigned to either of 10 groups, exposed to a specific stimulus material. For subjects in which number of classes increased, up to six, B-stimuli served as pictures, while A-, and Cstimuli were Greek letters, and "equivalence" was tested in CA tests. The A-, B-, and C-stimuli were the same for all subjects in whom class members increased up to six, where D-, E- and Fstimuli were Greek letters. FOllowing AB, BC, CD, DE, and FE training , FA, EA, FB, FC, EB, DA, FD, EC, DB, and CA "equivalence" tests were run. In the second experiment, a manyto-one training structure was used to study equivalence as a function of increasing class size without increase number of nodes. The results indicate that the probability of equivalence decreased more as function the number of nodes than as a function of number of classes. Reaction times, particularly to the comparison stimuli, generally increased initially during tests, possibly indicating precurrent problem solving behavior prior to the response to a comparison stimulus. Equivalent stimulus classes include relations not explicitly trained. The equivalence relation is defined by reflexivity, symmetry, and transitivity (e.g., Sidman & Tailby, 1982). Three different training structures have been used, linear series, many-to-one (comparison as node) and one-to-many (sample as node) to study equivalence (e.g., Green & Saunders, 1998). Stimulus equivalence has been consistently demonstrated in language-able human subjects, using a variety of stimulus materials (e.g., Barnes, McCullagh, & Keenan, 1990; Devany, Thanks to Lanny Fields for comments on parts of an earlier version of the manuscript and we also thank Dermot Barnes and an anonymous reviewer for helpful suggestions. Correspondence and reprints requests may be sent to Erik Arntzen, Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, PO Box 1094 Blindern, 0317 Oslo, Norway. (E-mail: Erik.Arntzen@psykologLuio.no). 80 ARNTZEN AND HOLTH Hayes, & Nelson, 1986; Fields, Reeve, Rosen, Varelas, Adams, Belanich, & Hobbie, 1997; Sidman, Kirk, & Willson-Morris, 1985; Sidman & Tailby, 1982; Sidman, Willson-Morris, & Kirk, 1986). However, equivalence classes in adults are unlikely to emerge following a linear series training structure when all baseline relations are trained concurrently and all probes for emergent relations are then introduced concurrently (Fields et aI., 1997). Similar results with 3 three-member stimulus classes were found by Arntzen and Holth (1997) when comparing percentages of subjects responding in accord with equivalence following linear series, many-to-one, and one-to-many training structure. Following a linear series training structure, only 2-3 of 10 subjects responded in accord with a positive equivalence test. Because the probability of a positive equivalence outcome is low after linear series training, such a structure is suitable for identification of historical variables that enhance equivalence class formation by adults (Fields et aI., 1997). The use of familiar stimulus material could obviously affect the probability of equivalence outcome. One study (Holth & Arntzen, 1998a) was concerned with effects of familiar stimuli on the rates of equivalence formation in a linear series training structure. The results showed that the probability of equivalence was low when the stimulus material consisted only of Greek letters. For the remaining groups, the probability of equivalence varied considerably depending upon whether the A-, B-, and/or C-stimuli were pictures, and 10 of 10 subjects responded in accord with equivalence when pictures served as B-stimuli. The results indicated that such a training structure may be suitable for studying effects of class size and number of classes on the formation of stimulus equivalence. Fields, Verhave, and Fath (1984) have predicted that there is an inverse relation between number of nodes and measures of response strength (accuracy and speed), and that the number of nodes may differentially affect the order in which derived relations come to exert stimulus control. Furthermore, derived relations would emerge differentially depending upon the number of nodes between stimuli during training. Similarly, Kennedy (1991) argued that derived relations emerge only after stimulus control by other derived relations with fewer nodes has been established , and less complex derived relations could be prerequisites for more complex derived relations. Saunders, Wachter, and Spradlin (1988) and Sidman et al. (1985) have both reported disruption in the emergence of equivalence classes that seems to be a function of the number of nodes separating stimuli. Furthermore, the results indicated that for some subjects control by stimuli separated by several nodes tended to emerge only after untrained relations separated by fewer nodes were established. Similarly, Haring, Breen, and Laitinen (1989) and Kennedy and Laitinen (1988) have reported disruptions in subject performances that may be a function of nodal number. Kennedy (1991) showed that in 2 seven-member stimulus classes the number of nodes separating stimuli influenced the emergence of derived relations in the formation of equivalence classes. CLASS SIZE VS. NUMBER OF CLASSES 81 Also, the same effect was obtained with 3 seven-member stimulus classes. However, Sidman (1994) pointed out that with a higher number of comparison stimuli, the influence of nodal number on probability of equivalence class formation is less obvious, and he called for experiments with three or more comparison stimuli per trial. Spencer and Chase (1996) argued that measures of response speed are important because they provide a more thorough understanding of the substitutability of stimuli in an equivalence class than do accuracy measures alone. Class formation is influenced by the training structures, the structure of the class as governed by the relations trained as the prerequisites for the classes, the familiarity of stimuli that are members of the potential classes, and the nodal structure of the class. A variable that has not yet been explored is the effect of number of classes on class formation. In the current experiment, the combined effects of number of classes and class size will be explored. This will be done by using a linear series training structure with familiar stimuli as B-stimuli. In addition to an index of equivalence, measures relevant to the readiness of class formation include reaction times to comparison stimuli and a measure of the consistency of responding during testing. Experiment 1 Method Subjects Fifty adults served as subjects, university or college students, and were successively assigned to 10 different experimental groups. The subjects were familiar with neither the stimulus equivalence research nor the stimulus material presented, except the pictures. Apparatus The apparatus was the same as described in Arntzen and Holth (1997). Procedure Stimulus material. Visual stimuli were displayed on the monitor. Greek letters and Cyrillic symbols were used in the experiment (see Table 1). Furthermore, the pictures used as B-stimuli are shown in Table 2. The presentation of the sample stimulus was always in the left-hand key (7 x 7 cm) of the monitor. Six comparison stimulus keys (4 x 4 cm) were arranged in two columns and three rows on the right-hand side of the monitor. As class size increases in a linear series training structure, the number of nodes in the class also increase as a function of class size. If class size is N, number of nodes will be N-2 (Fields & Verhave, 1987; Fields et aI., 1984). General information to the subjects. When asked to join the experiment, the subjects were told that the experiment was within the field of learning and was concerned with tasks presented on a computer with a touch ARNTZEN AND HOLTH 82 Table 1 Stimuli Used in Experiment 1 Classes Class members A 0 2 3 4 5 6 <p A 1K eJ 3 B Pictures C 'P ( 8 V ~ D P a 11 3 .!l E Y 11 ~ ~ F .!l ts ~ Table 2 Pictures Used as B-stimuli --. , <~~ ~-- ; Note. The number of pictures was dependent upon how many classes were used, but always included the three pictures in the upper row. 83 CLASS SIZE VS. NUMBER OF CLASSES screen. They were also told that the experiment would last for approximately 60 min, depending on how rapidly and correctly they responded. Instruction. The experimenter gave the following instruction: When you touch the left-hand stimulus, one or more stimuli will appear on the right. A touch on the correct stimulus will be followed by music from the cassette player, and incorrect responses will be followed by the blanking of the screen for 5 s before a stimulus in the left-hand key is presented again. Each part of the training requires a certain number of correct responses before proceeding to the next part. The training will be followed by tests, in which there will be no different consequences for correct and incorrect responses - no music and no blank screen. Training and test. Each trial started with the presentation of a sample stimulus. A touch on the sample stimulus was followed by the presentation of comparison stimuli in the keys on the right side of the monitor. The sample remained until a comparison stimulus was touched. To minimize the number of errors initially during training, the conditional discrimination tasks were introduced step by step: When a sample stimulus appeared for the first time during training, a touch on the sample stimulus was followed by the presentation of the correct comparison stimulus only. Next, each correct comparison was presented together with one incorrect comparison, then with the second incorrect comparison, and this was gradually increased until all comparison stimuli were presented. The comparison stimuli appeared in a random position from trial to trial. Both AB training and BC training required the successive correct completion of the randomly intermixed seven trials of each type before testing. For the subjects in which class size increased also CD, DE, and EF training required the successive correct completion of the Table 3 Stimulus Relations Trained for Each Class Size and Number of Classes Classes Members 4 3 4 5 A1B1 , A2B2, A.'lB3 B1C1, B2C2, B3C3 A1B1 , A2B2, A.'lB3, A4B4 A1B1 , A2B2, A.'lB3, A4B4, A5B5 B1C1, B2C2, B3C3, B4C4, B1C1, B2C2, B3C3, B4C4, B5C5 A1B1 , A2B2, A.'lB3 B1C1, B2C2, B3C3 C1D1 , C2D2, C3D3 A1B1 , A2B2, A.'lB3, A4B4 A1B1 , A2B2, A.'lB3, A4B4, A5B5 B1C1, B2C2, B3C3, B4C4 B1C1, B2C2, B3C3, B4C4, B5C5 C1D1 , C2D2, C3D3, C404 C1D1 , C2D2, C3D3, C404, C5D5 A1B1, A2B2, A.'lB3 B1Cl, B2C2, B3C3 C1 01 , C2D2, C3D3 01E1 , D2E2, D3E3 A1B1, A2B2, A.'lB3, A4B4 B1C1, B2C2, B3C3, B4C4 C1D1 , C2D2, C3D3, C404 01E1 , D2E2, D3E3, D4E4 A1B1 , A2B2, A.'lB3 B1C1, B2C2, B3C3 C1D1 , C2D2, C3D3 01E1 , D2E2, 03E3 E1 Fl , E2F2, E3F3 6 A1B1 , A2B2, A.'lB3, A4B4, A5B5, A6B6 B1C1, B2C2, B3C3, B4C4, B5C5, B6C6 84 ARNTZEN AND HOLTH randomly intermixed of seven trials of each type before testing. The equivalence tests were organized as two consecutive blocks with four of each trial type in each test block. For subjects in groups in which the class size increased the test with the largest number of nodes was given first, and tests which included pictures at the end. The experimental conditions for the 10 groups are summarized in Table 3. Dependent measures. Key presses on the touch screen in front of the monitor, reaction times, and the number of trials to criterion were recorded. An index of equivalence was calculated for each subject on each test half by dividing number of "correcf' responses by the total number of trials during each phase of the test. Equivalence was defined as an index of 0.9 or 1.0. For each subject who did not respond in accord with equivalence, consistency of responding was evaluated throughout the equivalence test. Similar to a "moving average," consistency indices were calculated for each block of six consecutive trials as described in Holth and Arntzen (1998a). In the group with 3 three-member stimulus classes, each time a sample occasioned a comparison selection consistent with a previous choice within the six-trial block, the consistency index would increase by 0.33. In the group with 4 four-member stimulus classes the consistency index was calculated for each block of eight consecutive trials. For each time a sample occasioned a comparison selection consistent with a previous choice within the eight-trial block, the consistency index would increase by 0.25. Furthermore, in the group with 6 three-member stimulus classes the consistency index was calculated for each block of 12 consecutive trials. For each time a sample occasioned a comparison selection consistent with a previous choice within the 12-trial block, the consistency index would increase by 0.17. Results The probability of equivalence-class formation was significantly more disrupted when the class size increased than when number of classes increased, as shown in Figure 1. In Groups 1, 2, and 3, with 3, 4, and 5 three-member stimulus classes respectively, all subjects responded in accord with equivalence (see Table 4), while in Group 4, with 6 three-member stimulus classes, 2 of 5 subjects responded in accord with equivalence in the first half and 1 more in the second half. In Group 5 with 3 four-member stimulus classes, 3 of 5 subjects responded in accord with equivalence in the DA test (two nodes), as shown in Table 5. In addition 1 subject responded with a positive CA test (one node) and additionally 1 subject in the DB test (one node). When number of classes increased to four (Group 6), 1 subject responded in accord with equivalence in the DA test. All 5 subjects responded in accord with equivalence in the DB test, and 1 of them (#785) did not in the CA test. When the number of classes further increased to five (Group 7),3 of CLASS SIZE VS. NUMBER OF CLASSES 85 percent 3 members 3 classes Figure 1. The figure shows the number of subjects who responded in accord with equivalence when either number of classes or nodes increased or both. Table 4 Total Number of Training Trials to Criterion and Indices for Equivalence Tests for Each Subject in the Different Groups No. classes Subj. # Total number of training trials Equivalence indices in the CA test Second half First half 3 321 322 323 324 325 132 117 121 100 130 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 4 751 752 753 754 755 187 240 206 155 163 0.9 0.9 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 5 761 762 763 764 765 284 429 521 309 321 0.8 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 0.9 0.9 1.0 1.0 1.0 6 771 772 773 774 775 453 320 402 272 981 0.8 0.3 1.0 0.9 0.7 0.9 0.4 1.0 0.9 0.7 Note. Number of classes are Indicated by the numbers in the left column. ARNTZEN AND HOLTH 86 5 subjects responded in accord with equivalence in the DA test, and furthermore, 1 subject responded in accord with equivalence in the two 1node tests. Table 5 Total Number of Training Trials to Criterion and Indices for Different Equivalence Tests for Each Subject in Groups with Four Members with an Increasing Number of Stimulus Classes #Equivalence tests 3 members DB Subj. 4 members DA Number of classes 3 4 5 731 732 733 734 735 CA 1 2 1 2 1 2 0.9 1.0 1.0 1.0 0.4 0.5 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 0.8 1.0 1.0 0.3 0.3 781 782 783 784 785 0.7 0.8 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.4 1.0 0.7 0.9 1.0 1.0 1.0 0.9 1.0 0.9 806 807 808 809 810 0.2 0.2 0.2 1.0 0.9 0.9 1.0 0.9 0.9 0.3 0.4 1.0 1.0 0.9 0.9 0.3 0.3 0.9 0.9 0.9 1.0 1.0 1.0 0.9 0.8 0.8 0.2 0.1 0.1 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 0.9 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 0.9 1.0 1.0 Note. Because of a programming error Subjects #731 and #781 received only one CA test. As shown in upper panel of Table 6, the group with 3 five-member stimulus classes, 1 (#744) of 5 subjects responded in accord with equivalence. Furthermore, 2 subjects responded with positive CA tests in Table 6 Indices for Different Equivalence Tests for Each Subject in Groups with Five Members with an Increasing Number of Stimulus Classes Subj# 5 members EA No. of classes 1 3 4 2 4 members EB DA 2 Equivalence tests 3 members EC DB 2 2 1 2 2 741 742 743 744 745 0.3 0.6 0.6 0.3 0.2 0.3 0.5 0.3 0.0 0.5 0.1 0.0 0.7 0.6 0.5 0.4 0.2 0.2 0.2 0.2 0.4 0.2 0.1 0.5 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 0.3 0.4 0.3 0.3 0.3 0.2 0.3 0.3 0.3 0.3 1.0 1.0 0.8 0.9 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 0.8 1.0 801 802 803 804 805 0.5 0.5 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 0.8 0.8 0.5 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 0.3 0.3 0.3 0.3 0.3 0.5 0.0 0.0 0.6 0.5 0.3 0.3 0.4 0.4 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.3 0.3 0.5 0.5 0.4 0.5 0.9 1.0 0.0 0.0 0.4 0.3 0.5 0.6 Note. #805 refused to complete the experiment. 0.9 1.0 CA 0.5 0.5 O. 87 CLASS SIZE VS. NUMBER OF CLASSES Table 7 Indices for Equivalence Tests for Each Subject in Group with 3 Six-Member Classes Subj. # 6 members FA Equivalence tests 4 members FC EB DA 5 members EA FB 12 FD EC 3 members DB CA 21212121212121212 791 1.0 1.0 0.3 0.3 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 0.7 0.7 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 792 1.0 1.0 0.9 0.9 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 793 0.8 1.0 0.9 0.8 1.0 1.0 0.8 0.9 1.0 0.9 0.3 0.4 0.4 0.4 0.9 1.0 0.3 0.3 0.9 1.0 794 0.4 0.1 0.3 0.3 - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - 795 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.1 0.3 0.3 0.3 0.3 1.0 1.0 0.0 0.1 0.3 0.3 0.3 0.3 1.0 1.0 0.0 0.1 Note. Subject #794 refused to complete the experiment. the first test half, and also the last two in the second test half. Two subjects responded with positive DB-tests. In Group 9, the bottom panel, 1 (#805) of 5 subjects responded in accord with equivalence in all tests, and subject (#801) responded in accord with equivalence in one of the 2nodes test (EB) and during the three 1-node tests. As shown in Table 7, in Group 10, with 3 six-member stimulus classes, 2 of 6 subjects (#792 and 796) responded in accord with equivalence during all tests. Subject #791 responded with indices of 1.0 6 three-member stimulus classes CA test OJ) 1.0 0 0- 0.83 c:: :.ec:: ~ Q) I-; ..... ...... (.) ~ S 0.67 Q) ...... ~ >. ~ ...... 0 B# 771 +# 772 ... #775 0.5 >< Q) '"0 ,s 0.0 0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 Moving 12-trial blocks Figure 2. Individual curves for each subject who did not respond in accord with equivalence in the group with 6 three-member stimulus classes. 88 ARNTZEN AND HOLTH or 0.9 in 8 of the 10 tests, and Subject #793 had indices of 1.0 or 0.9 in 7 of the 10 tests, and Subject #795 had indices of 1.0 or 0.9 in 2 of the 10 tests. Subject #794 failed in the two first tests and refused to participate further in the experiment. For the group with 6 three-member stimulus classes, the data for consistency indices throughout testing for subjects who did not respond in accord with equivalence showed that 1 subject (#771) responded consistently throughout the CA test, and the 2 others responded partly consistently (see Figure 2). (The data for 5 four-member, 4 five-member, and 3-six-member stimulus classes are not presented). 3 four-member stimulus classes :\-••0 "07,0., •• j V DA test 1.0 ••••••• 0.67 0.33 +# 733 0.0 1-6 5-10 -A 10-15 # 735 15-20 19-24 CA test 1.0 o 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0.67 0.33 B#735 0.0 1-6 5-10 10-15 15-20 19-24 Moving 6-trial blocks Figure 3. Individual curves for each subject who did not respond in accord with equivalence in the group with 3 three-member stimulus classes. 89 CLASS SIZE VS. NUMBER OF CLASSES As shown in Figure 3, for the group with 3 four-member stimulus classes, 1 subject responded consistently during the whole DA test, and the other subject responded partly consistently. The only subject, who did not respond in accord with equivalence, did respond consistently during the CA test. In the group with 4 four-member stimulus classes, subjects responded more consistently during the test, and 1 subject responded consistently during the whole CA test, as shown in Figure 4. 4 four-member stimulus classes DA test 1.0 +#781 .,. # 782 0.75 0.5 +#783 B#785 0.25 0.0 o 5 10 15 25 20 CA test 1.0 000000000000000000000000 0.75 B#785 0.5 0.25 0.0 o 5 10 15 20 25 Moving 8-trial blocks Figure 4. Individual curves for each subject who did not respond in accord with equivalence in the group with 4 four-member stimulus classes. The group with 3 five-member stimulus classes showed that consistency of responding was higher in the DA test than in the EA, EC, EB, and DB test (see Figure 5). Furthermore, a pattern of more consistent responding during the test was seen only in the DA test. 90 ARNTZEN AND HOLTH 3 five-member stimulus classes EA test *#741 ·10#742 *#743 B#745 1-6 5-10 10-15 15-20 19-24 DA test 1 .0 0 . 67 0 . 33 *#741 l~::::~ ·10#742 *#743 B#745 0 .0 1-6 5-10 10-15 19-24 15-20 EC test *#741 ·10#742 *#743 B#745 1-6 10-15 5-10 19-24 15-20 EB test *#741 1.0 ·10#742 0 . 67 *#743 B#745 0 . 33 0.0 1-6 5-10 10-15 15-20 19-24 DB test 1 .0 DDDD.iII,D\ 0 . 67 1o.·IoA 10 ·10#742 100000 B#745 -0000- .10. 0 . 33 0 .0 1-6 5-10 10-15 15-20 19-24 Moving 6-trial blocks Figure 5. Individual curves for each subject who did not respond in accord with equivalence in the group with 3 five-member stimulus classes. . CLASS SIZE VS. NUMBER OF CLASSES 91 3 three-member stimulus classes 16 CAtest Training sample ...... comparison 00 12 8 ~ 4 0 ~ 4 three-member stimulus classes 16 Training CA test 12 8 en -C C oC,.) ~ ~.IiI " ~ "1iI 4 O~~~~L-~--~~~--~~~~ 5 three-member stimulus classes CD en CA test Training o 6 three-member stimulus classes 16 Training CA test 12 8 4 0 ~ Last five ~ ~ 1 234 6 42 0" "0 44 46 trials Figure 6. Mean reaction times for subjects in all groups when increasing number of classes. In each graph the last five training trials followed by the first five test trials and the last five test trials. Reaction times to the sample stimuli is shown as open squares and reaction times to the comparison stimuli are shown as filled circles. 92 ARNTZEN AND HOLTH 3 four-member stimulus classes 18 15 12 9 6 3 0 Tr DA DB eN \4 CA ,'- 4 four-member stimulus classes Tr en "0 I=: 0 (.) (1) CI) 18 15 12 9 6 3 0 DB DA CA ..... eJ- 5 four-member stimulus classes Tr DB DA CA 18 15 12 9 6 3 0 .... Last five V1 5 Last five ~ 1m ..... 1 5 Last five 1 5 Last five Figure 7. Mean reaction times for subjects when increasing number of classes in the fourmember groups. In each graph the last five training trials followed by the first five test trials and the last five test trials. Reaction times to the comparison stimuli is shown as filled circles in the different "equivalence tests." 93 CLASS SIZE VS. NUMBER OF CLASSES In all groups there was a marked increase in reaction times to comparison stimuli from the final phase of training to the initial phase of the test, as shown in Figures 6, 7, 8, and 9. There was also a significant decrease in reaction times from the initial five test trials to the last five test trials for all groups. There was no difference in reaction time when comparing the final phase of training and the final phase of the test within each group. 3 five-member stimulus classes 16 Tr EA EB DA EC DB CA 12 en "d s:: u 0 8 ~ CI,) 4 0 ~ 1': ---':-.~~~_\-' 4 five-member stimulus classes 16 Tr EA EB DA EC DB CA 12 8 4 0 ~ Last five \\.: 1 5 Last 1 5 Last 1 5 Last 1 5 Last five five five five -~~ 1 5 Last five 1 5 Last five Figure 8. Mean reaction times for subjects when increasing number of classes in the fivemember groups. In each graph the last five training trials followed by the first five test trials and the last five test trials. Reaction times to the comparison stimuli is shown as filled circles in the different "equivalence tests." 94 ARNTZEN AND HOLTH Tr FA 16 14 12 ~ 10 § 8 ~ ~ 6 4 2 o EA FB FC EB DA FD EC DB CA ! ~li)I~I .uv.~~~~~~ Tests Figure 9. Mean reaction times for subjects in the 3 six-member group. In each graph the last five training trials followed by the first five test trials and the last five test trials. Reaction times to the comparison stimuli is shown as filled circles in the different "equivalence tests." Discussion The probability of equivalence class formation was significantly more disrupted when class size increased than when number of classes were increased. Consistency indices showed that most of the subjects tended to respond more systematically throughout testing whether or not they responded in accord with equivalence. However, even patterns of consistent responding that were not consistent with the predicted equivalence pattern were more disrupted when class size increased than when number of classes increased. Since consistency within trial types must be easier with fewer choices, the lower consistency with fewer choices, but more members, indicates that the tendency for consistency extends across nodal numbers. It should be noted that in two-choice matching-to-sample tasks, a consistent choice of a different comparison for each sample would, by chance, produce responding in accord with equivalence in 50% of the cases. This percentage is lower as the number of comparisons increases. In all groups in which there are three classes there is a 0.17 probability for equivalence to emerge by chance; while for instance, in 6 three-member stimulus classes the probability of 'equivalence'-by-chance is little more than one per thousand. To explain the differences in equivalence outcome in the present experiment, five different variables could be set forth. First, Sidman (1994) suggested that the change from simultaneous to successive discrimination or vice versa (e.g. , from training to test) as relevant in explaining the diverging outcomes of the different training structures, in terms of the probability of class formation. However, the change CLASS SIZE VS. NUMBER OF CLASSES 95 Table 8 Number of Successive or Simultaneous Discriminations as Function of Class Size and Number of Classes No. of classes No. of members Total no. of discriminations 3 4 5 6 3 3 3 3 36 66 105 153 3 4 5 4 4 4 3 4 3 No. of discriminations required Equivalence outcome in percent Ditt. between total and required discriminations No. of discriminations during each test 21 42 70 105 15 24 35 48 6 8 10 12 100 100 100 60 66 120 190 30 60 100 36 60 90 6 8 10 60 20 60 5 5 105 190 39 78 66 112 6 8 20 20 6 153 48 105 ·6 20 Note. The number of stimuli which must be discriminated (successively and .3jmultaneously) needed in each condition were calculated according to the following formula: M~/C-i) + C(C-1) (M-1). C is number of classes and M is number of members. The first part of the formula is for calculating the number of discriminations between classes, and the last part is for calculating number of discriminations within each stimulus set. Total number of discriminations in each condition is calculated according to the following formula fY. x Ivf2 - CM2. from training to test with respect to the switch in discrimination could not be a critical variable in explaining the results, because the equivalence outcome is highest when the number of classes increases, in which also the number of stimuli discriminated is much higher, compared to the number of stimuli discriminated when class size increases (see Table 8). Second, the total number of stimuli presented in each condition cannot explain the differences either, because the rate of equivalence is higher in the 6 three-member stimulus classes than in the 3 six-member stimulus classes. Third, increasing the number of classes may produce lower yields of equivalence outcome than a comparable increase in the number of members simply as a function of the higher discrepancy between the number of discriminations required to discriminate all the stimuli from each other stimulus and the number of discriminations actually required during training. This is in accord with the discrimination analysis set forth by Saunders and Green (1999). Fourth, Fields and coworkers (Fields, Adams, & Verhave, 1993) have set forth the idea that nodal number could also be an essential variable affecting the equivalence outcome. In the current experiment, number of nodes could be an important factor in explaining the differences in disruption of equivalence when either increasing size or classes. In the Kennedy (1991) study the greatest differences with respect to equivalence outcome seemed to occur between one and two nodes. In contrast, Sidman (1994) maintained that, with repeated testing, equivalence has eventually been demonstrated regardless of 96 ARNTZEN AND HOLTH nodal number and this might rather be a question of delayed emergence. However, as shown in the present study and elsewhere (Holth & Arntzen, 1998a, in press), subjects sometimes respond consistently with a pattern other than the predicted equivalence pattern. Fifth, number of familiar stimuli could be an important factor in explaining these results, because increasing number of classes in Experiment 1 involves an increasing number of familiar stimuli, while when increasing class size the number of familiar stimuli is constant, and as can be shown in Table 8 the equivalence outcome is highest in the case of increasing class number. This variable is linked to the fact that when increasing either the number of classes or the class size, the number of stimuli that are simultaneously or successively discriminated are increased differently with respect to training sequences. If the number of classes is increased by one, for example from a 4 to 5 threemember class, the number of stimuli that has to be simultaneously discriminated is increased by one in each trial type. On balance, if the number of members is increased by one, for example from a 4- to 5-member class, the number of stimuli that are simultaneously discriminated is constant. As in all stimulus equivalence experiments when a linear series training structure is used, the effect of number of nodes and class size are entangled. To separate the effects of these variables and to see if the difference between class size and number of classes is dependent on number of familiar stimuli, a second experiment included a many-to-one training structure, in which class size is increased. Obviously, increasing class size in a many-to-one training structure does not influence the number-of-nodes variable. Experiment 2 Method Subjects The subjects were 16 staff-members from a treatment center for retarded and autistic children, and teachers from a school for'youths with behavioral problems. They were successively assigned to four different experimental groups. The subjects were familiar neither with stimulus equivalence research nor with the stimulus material presented, except for the pictures. Apparatus and Procedure were the same as in the preceding experiment. The experimental conditions for the four groups are summarized in Table 9. Table 9 Stimulus Relations Trained for Group Depending on Class Size Members 2 3 4 Stimulus relations A1B1 , A2B2, A3B3, C1B1 , C2B2,C3B3 A1B1 , A2B2, A3B3, C1B1 , C2B2, C3B3,D1B1,D2B2,D3B3 A1B1 , A2B2, A3B3, C1B1 , C2B2, C3B3, D1B1,D2B2,D3B3,E1B1 , E2B2, E3B3 A1B1 , A2B2, A3B3, C1B1 , C2B2, C3B3, D1B1,D2B2, D3B3, E1B1 , E2B2,E3B3, F1B1,F2B2,F3B3 97 CLASS SIZE VS. NUMBER OF CLASSES Results The probability of an equivalence outcome decreased as a function of increasing class size, as shown in Figure 10. In the groups with three and four members, all subjects responded in accord with equivalence, and when the number of members was increased to five and six, 75% and 50%, respectively, of the subjects responded in accord with equivalence. 100 80 60 40 20 o-'---'----3 4 5 6 Number of members Increasing class size Figure 10. The figure shows the percentage of subjects who responded in accord with equivalence following the many-to-one structure. ARNTZEN AND HOLTH 98 As shown in Tables 10 and 11, all subjects who were exposed to three- and four-member classes responded in accord with equivalence. Table 10 Indices for Equivalence Test for Each Subject in Group with Three Members in Experiment 2 Subject Number Equivalence test CA 2 811 1.0 1.0 812 0.3 1.0 813 1.0 1.0 814 1.0 1.0 Table 11 Indices for Different Equivalence Tests for Each Subject in Group with Four Members in Experiment 2 Equivalence tests DC 2 Subject Number DA 1 2 816 1.0 1.0 1.0 CA 1 2 1.0 1.0 1.0 817 1.0 1.0 0.9 1.0 1.0 1.0 818 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 819 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 When class size was increased to five members, 3 of 4 subjects responded in accord with equivalence, as shown in Table 12. Subject #821 had indices of 1.0 on all tests except for the ED test, where he had indices of 0.8 and 0.6. Table 12 Indices for Different Equivalence Tests for Each Subject in Group with Five Members in Experiment 2 Subj# EA 1 2 DA 1 2 Equivalence tests EC ED 1 2 2 1 DC 2 CA 1 2 821 0.9 1.0 1.0 0.8 822 1.0 0.9 0.8 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 0.9 823 1.0 0.9 1.0 0.9 1.0 1.0 1.0 0.9 1.0 0.9 1.0 1.0 824 1.0 1.0 0.9 0.9 0.8 1.0 1.0 1.0 0.9 1.0 1.0 0.8 1.0 0.9 0.8 0.6 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 As shown in Table 13, 2 of 4 subjects which were exposed to training for the establishment of six-member classes responded in accord with equivalence. Subject #828 had indices of 1.0 on 9 of the 10 tests, and on the DA-test the indices were 0.8 and 0.7. Subject #827 had indices of 1.0 on all but two tests, the EA and DA tests. 99 CLASS SIZE VS. NUMBER OF CLASSES Table 13 Indices for Equivalence Tests for Each Subject in Group with 3 Six-Member Classes Subj. # FA EA FC DA Equivalence Tests FD EC FE ED DC CA 22222122222 826 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 827 0.8 0.9 0.7 0.8 1.0 1.0 0.8 0.8 0.9 1.0 0.8 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 0.8 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 828 1.00.9 0.9 1.0 1.00.9 0.8 0.7 1.0 1.0 0.8 1.0 0.9 1.0 1.0 0.9 0.9 1.0 1.0 1.0 829 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 0.9 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 I:~o 100 I 0\ o I-< o ~ ...... t) .-"" 80 ~ (.) '"d .S 60 ..... ""(.) :S ::l 40 "" 4-< o ~ 5 I:::: 20 ~ ~ tl.. o FA EA FC DA FD EC FE/ ED/ DC/ CA FB EBDB "Equivalence tests" Figure 11. The figure shows percentage of subjects with indices of 1.0 and 0.9 across the different "equivalence tests:' Subjects tested following the linear series structure are marked with filled bars and subjects tested following the many-to-one structure are marked with gray bars. ARNTZEN AND HOLTH 100 CA Bsl 9 6 3 ~ 0 9 rJ1 ~ ~ 5 Last five Bsl DA CA DC 6 0 () (l,) Last five :-J 3 r./'J j.. : 0 Last five 1 5 Bsl EA Last five DA 1 5 EC 1\ J 5 Last five ED Last five DC CA 9 6 3 o Last 1 5 Last 1 5 Last 1 5 Last 1 5 Last 1 5 Last 1 5 Last five five five five fIVe five five Figure 12. Mean reaction times for subjects when increasing number of members. In each graph the last five training trials followed by the first five test trials and the last five test trials. Reaction times to the comparison stimuli is shown as filled circles in the different "equivalence tests." 101 CLASS SIZE VS. NUMBER OF CLASSES The percentage of subjects who had indices of 1.0 or 0.9 on the different tests for equivalence where higher across all tests for subjects trained with the many-to-one structure with six-member classes compared to subjects trained with linear series structure, as shown in Figure 11 . Reaction times to comparison stimuli increased from the five last training trials to the first five test trials, as shown in Figures 12 and 13. Furthermore, there was no systematic decrease in reaction times across successive tests. 12 Bsl FA EA FC DA FD EC ED FE DC CA 10 00 8 "'0 ~ 0 U 6 ~ V) 4 Jv~~ W . 2 0 Last 1 5 Last 1 5 Last 1 five five five . 5 Last 1 5 Last 1 5 Last 1 5 Last 1 5 last 1 5 Last five five five fIVe five five !v.v 1 5 Last five 1 5 last five Figure 13. Mean reaction times for subjects when increasing number of members. In each graph the last five training trials followed by the first five test trials and the last five test trials. Reaction times to the comparison stimuli is shown as fi lled circles in the different "equivalence tests." Discussion When stimulus equivalence was tested in subjects after conditional discrimination training according to a many-to-one structure, all subjects did respond in accord with equivalence in the three- and four-member classes, and 3 and 2 of 4 subjects, respectively responded in accord with equivalence in the five- and six-member classes. The results indicate that the number of familiar stimuli could not be a critical factor in explaining the differences in equivalence outcome following either increasing number of classes or class size, because the equivalence outcome was about the same for the subjects following the many-to-one training structure as for the subjects following the linear series structure when number of classes was expanded. Although the test procedures used in the current experiments are sensitive to occasional errors, the percentage of subjects who responded in accord with equivalence was higher on all tests following the many-toone structure than following the linear series structure (see Figure 11). 102 ARNTZEN AND HOLTH General Discussion The purpose of the current study was to investigate the likelihood of equivalence class formation as a function of class size and number of classes. Because class size and nodal number are confounded in a linear series training structure, a many-to-one structure was included to separate these variables. The results from the current experiments showed that the equivalence outcome was decreased as a function of both number of classes and nodes, but the decrease was much more pronounced as a function of nodes. Furthermore, as shown in Experiment 2, in which class size was increased in a many-to-one structure, the equivalence outcome did not differ from the equivalence outcome when the number of classes increased as shown in Experiment 1. This indicates that equivalence class formation was not so much influenced by increasing class size as by an increasing number of nodes. The number of singles linked to a node is termed "nodal density" and is determined by the distribution of singles (Fields & Verhave, 1987). Therefore, increasing the number of singles linked to a nodal stimulus might inhibit or enhance the formation of emergent relations (Fields & Verhave, 1987). In a many-to-one training structure, nodal number is constant while the "nodal density" increases as a function of increasing class size. The differential outcome with respect to probability of class formation in the 6 three-member classes in Experiment 1 and 3 six-member classes in Experiment 2, could be a function of such "nodal density" with respect to lower equivalence outcome when increasing the nodal density. Although as suggested by Sidman (1994) equivalence might eventually emerge regardless of nodal number, this is clearly not always the case. The present finding of a gradual change in responding to become consistent with some pattern other than the predicted equivalence pattern was also evident in Holth and Arntzen (1998a). A history of differential reinforcement for consistent responding might explain the finding of improved performances during testing, that is, delayed emergence of stimulus equivalence and increased consistency of responding even when not in accord with experimenter-predicted equivalence performances. Bush, Sidman, and deRose (1989) found that the emergence of new conditional relations came about abruptly with repeated testing. The comparison selection on every trial becomes consistent with expected equivalence classes because the test trials provide no other basis for choice that is consistently available (Sidman, 1994). The gradual vs. abrupt emergence could be related to the fact that Sidman's analyses were based on testing for emergent relations in which the tests were arranged as test probes and not as separate test blocks as in the present study. The differential effects of conducting tests for the properties of equivalence arranged in test probes or separate test blocks are explored relatively little (Green & Saunders, 1998), and experiments should be designed to study these differences in test arrangements. The current study has replicated earlier findings in which an increase in reaction time from training to test, and a decrease during the test has been shown (Arntzen & Holth, 1997; Holth & Arntzen, 1998a, 1998b). Fields and Verhave (1987) have postulated that as nodal distance increases, accuracy of CLASS SIZE VS. NUMBER OF CLASSES 103 responding decreases. Reaction times are sensitive to different variables even when the accuracy of responding is not (Holth & Arntzen, in press; Spencer & Chase, 1996). In the Spencer and Chase (1996) study, response speed was inversely related to the number of nodes on which the tested relations were based. Furthermore, differences in accuracy across nodal distance and trial type were significant only on the first tests for equivalence, whereas differences in speed were significant even after extended testing. For example, during combined testing all subjects responded faster to one-node than to fivenode combined relations, and data from half of the subjects indicated an inverse relation. All subjects responded faster on one-node than on five-node transitivity relations. The results of the current experiment showed similar effects as a function of increasing nodes in which reaction times decreased across successive tests. Furthermore, such effects were not found following the many-to-one training structure in which class size was increased. During recent years Fields and coworkers have been concerned with variables influencing the likelihood of emergent stimulus classes. First, the formation of stimulus classes has been shown to be a direct function of number of nodes; the formation of stimulus classes is less likely as number of nodes increases (Fields et aL, 1993). Second, pretraining enhances the emergence of new stimulus classes, and the enhancement effect is a direct function of class size trained (Buffington, Fields, & Adams, 1997). Third, the number of stimuli in the pretrained classes enhance the emergence of new equivalence classes (Fields et aL, 1997). Finally, emergence of new equivalence classes have been shown to be a function of three training protocols, simple-to-complex, complex-ta-simple, and simultaneous protocols (Fields et aL, 1997). The current experiments have extended these findings, showing that the emergence of stimulus equivalence was more disturbed by number of nodes than both class size and number of stimulus classes. The results indicate that an increasing number of nodes, and not just increasing class size, inhibits an equivalence outcome to a greater extent than does an expanding number of classes. References ARNTZEN, E., & HOLTH, P. (1997). Probability of stimulus equivalence as a function of training design. The Psychological Record, 47, 309-320. BARNES, D., MCCULLAGH, P. D., & KEENAN, M. (1990). Equivalence class formation in non-hearing impaired children and hearing impaired children. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 8, 19-30. BUFFINGTON, D. M., FIELDS, L., & ADAMS, B. J. (1997). Enhancing equivalence class formation by pretraining of other equivalence classes. The Psychological Record, 47, 69-96. BUSH, K. M., SIDMAN, M., & DE ROSE, T. (1989). Contextual control of emergent equivalence relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 51, 2945. DEVANY, J. M., HAYES, S. C., & NELSON, R. 0. (1986). Equivalence class formation in language-able and language-disabled children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 46, 243-257. 104 ARNTZEN AND HOLTH FIELDS, L. , ADAMS, B. J., & VERHAVE, T. (1993). The effects of equivalence class structure on test performances. The Psychological Record, 43, 697-712. FIELDS, L. , REEVE, K. F. , ROSEN, D., VARELAS, A., ADAMS, B. J., BELANICH, J., & HOBBIE, S. A. (1997). Using the simultaneous protocol to study equivalence class formation: the facilitating effects of nodal number and size of previously established equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 67, 367-389. FIELDS, L., & VERHAVE, T. (1987). The structure of equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 48, 317-332. FIELDS, L., VERHAVE, T., & FATH, S. (1984). Stimulus equivalence and transitive associations: A methodological analysis. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 42,143-157. GREEN, G., & SAUNDERS, R. R. (1998). Stimulus equivalence. In K. A. Lattal & M. Perone (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in human operant behavior (pp. 229-262). New York: Plenum Press. HARING, T. G., BREEN, C. G., & LAITINEN, R. E. (1989). Stimulus class formation and concept learning: Establishment of within- and between-set generalization and transitive relationship via conditional discrimination procedures. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 52, 13-25. HOLTH, P., & ARNTZEN, E. (1998a). Stimulus familiarity and the delayed emergence of stimulus equivalence or consistent nonequivalence. The Psychological Record, 48, 81-110. HOLTH, P. , & ARNTZEN, E. (1998b). Symmetry versus sequentiality related to prior training , sequential dependency of stimuli, and verbal labeling. The Psychological Record, 48, 293-315. HOLTH, P., & ARNTZEN, E. (in press). Reaction times and the emergence of class consistent responding: A case for precurrent responding? KENNEDY, C. H. (1991). Equivalence class formation influenced by the number of nodes separating stimuli. Behavioural Processes, 24, 219-245. KENNEDY, C. H., & LAITINEN, R. E. (1988). Second-order control of symmetric and transitive stimulus relations: The influence of order effects. The Psychological Record, 38, 437-446. SAUNDERS, R. R. , & GREEN, G. (1999). A discrimination analysis of training-structure effects on stimulus equivalence outcomes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 72, 117-137. SAUNDERS, R. R., WACHTER, J. A. , & SPRADLIN, J. E. (1988). Establishing auditory stimulus control over an eight-member equivalence class via conditional discrimination procedure. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 49, 95-115. SIDMAN, M. (1994). Equivalence relations and behavior: A research story. Boston: Authors Cooperative. SIDMAN, M., KIRK, B., & WILLSON-MORRIS, M. (1985). Six members stimulus classes generated by conditional-discrimination procedures. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 43, 21-42. SIDMAN, M., & TAILBY, w. (1982). Conditional discrimination vs. matching to sample: An expansion of the testing paradigm. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 5-22. SIDMAN, M., WILLSON-MORRIS, M., & KIRK, B. (1986). Matching-to-sample procedures and the development of equivalence relations: The role of naming. Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 6, 1-29. SPENCER, T. J., & CHASE, P. N. (1996). Speed analysis of stimulus equivalence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 643-659.
© Copyright 2025 Paperzz