O�":"il:ts (' 'I"l' ,',; from ;'1 F,,11 " vior l-�:i �,: i", 'i :n��.; ��. i ( ' , 'PH R�l'SC and Frederick J. Ligbter !:li, I;) 19I9 by John Wih.:y & SOIlS. loco 9 1 SOCIAL BEHAVIOR IN SOME MARINE AND TERRESTRIAL CARNIVORES BURNEY J. LE BOEUF Crown College University of California, Santa Cruz 1 2 3 4 5 1 Introduction Morphological Comparisons Prey, Food Habits, and Feeding Behavior Social Behavior and Reproduction Summary 251 255 257 263 274 INTRODUCTION Once upon a time, around 37 million years ago, when primitive fissiped carnivores were evolving from their miascid predecessors, some of them forsook their way of life on land and went to sea to find food. This move must have worked out well, because many of the descendants of these 1 pioneers-the seals, sea lions, and walruses-are still making a living in the sea today. Of course, descendants of the carnivores that remained on land fared well, too. Because land and sea are such different environ ments, we might expect the two animal groups to have diverged in many ways since their separation. Indeed, it should be interesting to compare representatives of the two groups because of what they have in common, an ancestor, and because of the obvious differences in the environments in which they live. In this chapter, I point out some differ251 252 Social Behavior In Some Carnivores ences and similarities between seals and their terrestrial conterparts with respect to gross morphology, feeding and predatory behavior, reproduc tive behavior, and various aspects of social life. As is necessary when comparing two large groups of animals, I document points with exam ples from a few selected species. My selection is biased toward those species that have been studied most systematically, those that are of greatest theoretical interest, and those studies with which I am familiar. The similarities between seals and land carnivores were obvious to laymen and early scientists (Fig. 9.1). Fifteenth and 16th century sailors described the seals they saw as marine carnivores and called them "sea lions," "sea bears," or "Iobos de mar" (sea wolves). Linnaeus and sev eral later taxonomists classified seals, sea lions, and walruses in the order Carnivora. However, as early as 1811, there was an attempt to place these animals in a separate order (Illiger, 1811). At present, many investigators class these marine mammals in their own separate order, the Pinnipedia (e.g., King, 1964). However, classification of these ani mals is far from settled. Some maintain that the new order is inapprop riate and that pinnipeds should be considered merely a suborder of the Carnivora (McKenna, 1969). Others argue that the pinnipeds can be classified into two living families (walruses are placed in the same family as sea lions) under the superfamily, Canoidea, in the order Carnivora (Mitchell and Tedford, 1973). Before I make specific comparisons, it may be useful to present a sketch of pinniped evolutionary history. Their history, although poorly documented, may provide clues to differences we might expect between them and terrestrial carnivores. The earliest pinnipeds probably entered the sea in one of the following areas: the Arctic Basin, the northwest coast of Nor.th America, or the Tethyan-Mediterranean area (Matthew, 1939; Davies, 1958a; McLaren, 1960; Repenning, 1970). It is not clear whether seals and sea lions (includ. ing the walrus) were already differentiated at this time. The oldest pin niped remains; ar,e from the Miocene era, a time when they were already well adapted for aquatic life and when seals and sea lions were already distinguishable (Downs, 1956; Mitchell and Tedford, 1973). One opinion holds that all pinnipeds derived from canoid or dog-bear stock (e.g., Matthew, 1939; Simpson, 1945; Davis, 1958a; Sarich, 1969). An opposing view is that sea lions and the walrus derived from ursine stock, and seals derived from lutrine stock (e.g., Mivart, 1885; McLaren, 1960; King, 1964; Repenning, 1970). This question is far from settled. Nevertheless, there is agreement that all pinnipeds are more closely related phylogenetically to members of the superfamily Canoidea of the order Carnivora--:-the ,... Gi UI a: ::;) CJ ii: 253 254 Social Behavior In Some Carnivores dogs, raccoons, bears, weasels, and the Iike--than to the feloid carni vores. After they entered the water, the distribution and diversity of pin. nipeds were influenced by geomorphic and climatic barriers, distance between land falls, ocean currents, and water temperature (Davies, 1958a; King, 1964; Hendey, 1972). Davies (1958b) hypothesizes that pin nipeds are, and always have been, tied to a cold-water environment. He argues very persuasively that the distribution and differentiation of present-day northern pinnipeds reflects periods of expansion and con traction of sea and glacial ice that occurred during the Pleistocene era. Scheffer (1958, p. 37) points out that "as local shore lines and islands rose and fell and glacial barriers came and went, pinnipeds moved back and forth in order to maintain favorable breeding grounds along the edge of the sea." On land, many contemporary carnivores were being forced into extinction by periodic fires, floods, droughts, and dust storms. Scheffer suggests that the early pinnipeds moved out along the shores of continents, from island to island and later to polar ice fields. The rate of evolution was faster at the frontiers of the advancing lines where immigration was unidirectional. He speculates further: The primitive populations were small, composed of family groups, not very sociable. The members did not wander far, had no well-developed homing instinct, were not migratory, and did not need to rendezvous in special places in order to find mates. Polygyny had not developed in any of the pinniped stocks. The animals were perhaps smaller than most recent pinnipeds (Cope's rule), had less fat, lived in more temperate waters, and perhaps made crude dens in beach grasses and among boulders. The advancing pinnipeds met, from sea birds and cetaceans, little competition for food. They met no competition for breeding room, nor do they often today. Pinnipeds early lost the habit of feeding on beach organisms such as crabs, mussels, periwinkles, and blennies. [Scheffer, 1958, p. 36-371 As each generic stock became isolated, it was transformed in response to the local physical and biotic environment and adapted to its peculiar niche. Among species frequenting isolated bays, gulfs, islands, inland seas, or lakes, there has been, and still is, rapid evolution. In the last few centuries, the effect of commercial sealing upon island and continental pinniped populations has been catastrophic. Millions of seals have been slaughtered and entire breeding po pula tions wiped out. It is doubtful that some species will ever recover genetically from under going such a severe population "bottleneck" (Le Boeuf, 1977). In one recently exploited species, absolutely no polymorphic variation was Morphological Comparisons found in 255 21 blood proteins from 159 se<lls representing five rookeries 1974)! In this regard, some land carnivore popu (Bonnell and Selander, lations have suffered a similar f<lte at the hands of man-for example, the wolf. The increasing range of human populations is changing the distribution of both land carnivores and pinnipeds. Although pinnipeds are marine, all species have retained an attach ment to land. In this respect, they differ from the Cetacea, descendants of a primitive ungulate ancestor, who have become completely aquatic. All pinnipeds 'give birth on land or ice, and some species spend the majority of their time resting out of the water. Pinnipeds have exploited the marine niche for food. A few carnivores, such as the sea otter,' Enhydra /utris, and the polar bear, Thalarctos maritimus, also obtain food from the sea, but they have not undergone the same degree of morphological transformation to aquatic living as the pinnipeds. The anatomical and behavioral adaptations of the pinnipeds to life in the sea clearly distinguishes them from the land carnivores. Since these adaptations influence their behavior on land, it is worth reviewing some of the major ones. 2 MORPHOLOGICAL COMPARI�ONS Many morphological differences between pinnipeds and land carnivores are simply a reflection of the different environments in which they live. The average body size of pinnipeds is greater than that of ca!nivores. The smallest pinniped, the ringed seal, Pusa hispida, weighs 90 kg. The Mirounga leonina, weighs about 3,629 kg and is 650 cm long (Scheffer, 1958). Several seal species are larger than the largest land carnivore, the grizzly bear, Ursus arctos, which weighs 771 kg (Erdbrink, 1953). Many land carnivores, such a� the foxes of Africa (Bekoff, 1975), weigh less than 4 kg. The Fennec fox, Fennecus zerda, weighs less than 1 kg! The greater body size of pinnipeds largest pinniped, the southern elephant seal, evolved mainly in response to the marine environment. Large body size is b-etter for heat retention in the told sea. Moreover, since the water medium gives more support than air, large size could mote easily evolve in the sea than on land. The pinniped body is adapted for swimming and diving. Body shape is streamlined to reduce drag. The external ears are reduced or absent, external genitalia and mammary teats are drawn into the body, limbs are enclosed within the body and extremities are flattened-the tail is short and the head is flattened, and the eyes are situated well forward. The neck, in particular, is thick and muscular and considerably more flexible 256 Social Behavior In Some Carnivores than in most large carnivores. The skin is adapted to a water environ ment (Montagna and Harrison, 1957), and the hair is flattened. Pin nipeds never groom the pelage with the mouth or tongue (Scheffer, 1958). The terrestrial carnivore body is a picture of contrast. The canid body, especially the large one, is designed for long-distance running (see Romer, 1966; Mech, 1970; Fox, 1975) . Subcutaneous fat forms a substantial part of all pinniped bodies. Skin and blubber make up more than 25% of the weight of the Weddell seal, LeptonycllOtes weddell; (Bruce, 1951) and almost 50% of the body weight of the southern elephant seal (Laws, 1953). Blubber provides reserve energy during fasts and lactation, thermal insulation, buoyancy, and the padding necessary for a streamlined profile. The proportion of body fat to total weight in land carnivores is apparently much smaller, even in dormant or hibernating bears, skunks, and raccoons. Although the pinniped brain has not been studied in great detail, it is evidently large, like that of terrestrial carnivores, but more spherical and more highly convoluted (King, 1964). The sea lion brain resembles that of bears, while the seal brain looks more like that of cats and dogs. Compared to a dog's brain, the cerebellum is large, probably because of the increased coordination demanded in swimming. The auditory nerve is large and the olfactory lobes are reduced. Pinniped eyes (the walrus excepted) are larger than those of land carnivores, and they function well at low levels of illumination. Seal vision is excellent both in water and on land (Schusterman, 1972). Pinnipeds have fewer and more uniform teeth than most land carni vores (except for the walrus, a special case once again). All species have the pronounced upper and lower canines but lack the carnassial cusps characteristic of land carnivores. The postcanines in most species are rudimentary pegs functioning to hold prey that is swallowed whole. Variation in pinniped teeth reflects the type of flesh the animal feeds on, and, as in many mammals, it is a useful taxonomic indicator. Generally, the teeth, mouth, jaw, and associated structures of pinnipeds are de signed for grasping, tearing, and swallowing prey whole or in chunks, rather than for chewing, shearing, or crushing large bones. In contrast with land carnivores, the deciduous teeth of pinnipeds disappear before or soon after birth. Pinnipeds have made numerous physiological adjustments to aquatic life that set them apart from land carnivores. These adaptations in respi ration, circulation, renal physiology, and the like are important and interesting, but it would be too much of a digression to cover these topics here. Ridgeway (1972) provides an excellent review of marine mammal adaptation to the aquatic environment. 257 Prey, Food Habits, and Feeding Behavior 3 PREY, FOOD HABITS, AND FEEDING BEHAVIOR Pinnipeds deviated from land carnivores in form and function because of adaptations that took place as a result of seeking food in the sea. It is therefore most important to compare these two animal groups from the point of view of hunting and feeding habits, and in relation to their respective prey. Both pinnipeds and land carnivores are meat eaters, but only the marine mammals eat flesh exclusively. Seals and sea lions feed mainly on crustacea, molluscs, fish, penguins, and other sea birds. Although diet varies with the species, the majority of pinnipeds feed on a variety of ' prey. The Alaska fur seal, Callorizilllls ursillus, is the best studied pin niped from the point of view of food habits. Its catholic diet is represen tative of the wide range of fishes and squid eaten by the Otariids (the earred s,eals or sea lions and fur seals, as opposed to the Phocids, the earless, true seals). Alaska fur seals feed on more than 30 kinds of 27 species of fish, 1 species of octopus, and 5 species of squid (Fiscus, Niggol, and Wilke, 1961). The most common marine organisms: at least q prey of fur seals are northern anchovy, Ellgraulis lIIordax, s uid, and Pacific herring (C/lipea izamlgus). Various rockfish, Pacific hake, cius Prodllctlls, Pacific saury, Merluc Cololabis saira, salmon, OncorizYllclluS spp. , and American shad, Alosa sapidissimia, are eaten in lesser quantities. Hermit crabs, amphipods, and various diving sea birds (e. g. , Rhinocer ous Auklet, Ceror/lillen lIIollocerata, Red-throated Loon, Cavia stellata, and Beal Petre\' Oceal/odrollla leucorizoa beali) are eaten occasionally, but they seem 'to play only a minor role in the seals' diet (Niggol, Fiscus and Wilke, 1959) . Fur seals, like most other pinnipeds, are opportunistic feeders. What is eaten depends on the seasonal distribution and the abundance of prey. Since fur seals commonly feed on sc � ooling fishes, stomach con tents of several animals collected in the same area often contain only one type of, fish. The Steller sea lion, Eumetopias jubatq (Spaulding, 1964), a�d the California sea lion, Zalophus cali/ornianus (Fiscus and Baines, 1966), eat similar fishes and squid, especially where the feeding range of the ' three species overlaps. Steller males in the Bering Sea and off Alaskan : islands supplement their diet with Alaska fur seal pups (Roger Gentry, personal communication). Most true seals, such as the harbor seal, Phoca seal, vitulina, and the grey Halichoerus grypus, are mainly fish eaters, but they occ�sionally feed on an assortment of crustacea, octopus, eels, and molh,lscs (Spaulding, 1964; Kenyon, 1965; Anderson et al . , 1974) . The northern elephant seal, Mirouna angustirostris, is a deep diving seal that feeds primarily on squid, 258 Social Behavior In Some Carnivores skates, rays, small sharks, and ratfish, Hydrolages co lli er (Huey, 1930; 1970). The small ringed seal, Pusa Hispida, which inhabits the circumpolar Arctic coasts, feeds on up to 72 different species Morejohn and Baltz, of small pelagic amphipods, euphausians, and other crustacea, as well as on small fish (King, Only two of the (King, 1964). 32 different pinniped species in King's classification 1964) are rather specialized feeders that exploit one type of prey Lobodon carcinophagus, feeds species almost exclusively. The Crabeater, on krill, small shrimp-like animals that it catches in quantity and strains from the water through its multicusped cheek teeth, much as a mysticete whale sieves krill through its baleen. The walrus, Odobenus rosmarus, Mya, Saxicava, and Cardium, for which it forages in shallow coastal waters less than 40 feeds primarily on three genera of bivalve molluscs, fathoms deep. Bivalves on the sea bottom are examined and sorted by the lips and whiskers; feet and fleshy parts are torn off and swallowed whole or sucked out. But when molluscs are scarce, the walrus may eat young ringed seals, bearded seals, Erignathus barbatus, and even young walruses. It is the only pinniped that apparently feeds on Cetacea. Occa sionally a narwhal, Monodon monoceros, or a beluga, Delphinapterus leucas is eaten. However, it is not known whether walruses acturally kill them or feed on corpses (King, 1964). The diversity of foods eaten by canids and ursids is even greater than that of pinnipeds. According to Mech (1970), "probably every kind of backboned animal that lives in the range of the wolf has been eaten by the wolf." Wolves, Canis lupus, eat mice, mink, muskrats, squirrels, rabbits, various birds, fish, lizards, snakes, grasshoppers, earthworms, and berries. But predation on small animals plays only a minor role in the wolf's diet. The main prey of the wolf, like those of the African wild dog, Lymoll pictlls, are large animals. The wolf's primary prey in the United States and Canada are: white-tailed deer, mule deer, moose, caribou, elk, Dall sheep, bighorn sheep, and beaver. Wild dogs kill mostly Thomson's gazelles, juvenile wildebeests and Grant's gazelles (Estes and Goddard, 1967), and, occasionally, warthogs and zebras (van 1971). African foxes feed Lawick-Goodall and van Lawick-Goodall, mostly on rodents and small reptiles, birds's eggs, insects and vegetable matter; jackals eat similar foods in addition to small mammals and carr ion (Bekoff, in Fox 1975). Additional information on other canids can be found (1975). Of all the land carnivores, bears have the most diverse tastes. The grizzly, before man virtually annihilated it, was an omnivorous oppor tunist par excellence . The California grizzly (Storer and Tevis, 1955) ate almost anything and everything that was available. This long list in- 259 Prey, Food Habits, and Feedi ng Behavior cluded: meat, fresh or putrid from whales, water birds, fish, elk, deer, antelope, gophers, lizards, frogs, and domestic livestock; a wider variety of plant materials than herbivorous animals ate, some of which were various berries, clovers, nuts, wheat, corn, potatoes, tree bark, and vari ous bulbs; and assorted foods like honey, ants and their larvae, yellow jacket nests, and mushrooms. Polar bears, Tlwlarctos maritimus, have a much mQre restricted diet. Although they consume some vegetation and carrion during the summer (Russell, 1975), they feed primarily on the 1975). ringed seal during most of the year (Stirling and McEwan, The remarkable thing about the feeding habits of the large canids is that the prey is often larger than the predator. In this respect, the large canids differ from all of the pinnipeds. Wild dogs, whose average weight does not exceed 18 kg, bring down impala and reedbucks that are double 1967) and zebras that are even larger. Wolves weighing 36 to 45 kg kill moose and bison that may weigh over 500 kg. These canids overcome large prey by hllnting in packs that or triple their size (Estes and Goddard, range from a few animals up to a score or more of. them. One dog usually selects a quarry from a retreating herd of g"zelles, and the other dogs follow it. The quarry is coursed for 1 to 3 km until it becomes exhausted. The lead dog catches up to it and grabs it or bowls it over. Once overta ken, the prey is fallen on by all the following dogs, and it is dismem bered and eaten with great dispatch. Wolves bring down deer or moose in a similar way (Mech, 1970). The first wolf to catch up to the fleeing prey attempts to grab hold of the rump, flanks, neck, or nose. Once the prey is down, others attack from every side. Although the technique may vary with prey species, the strategy of the individual quarry, and the composition of the hunting pack, it is clear that these pack hunters get help from each other in getting their food. Lone wolves or even small packs are at a disadvantage in bringing down large prey. In black backed jackals, Canis mesome/as,. Wyman (1967) observed that two hun ters were more than four times as successful as one in bringing down gazelle fawns. , Like land carnivores, the size of prey eaten by pinnipeds varies greatly, but, except for the dubious possibility that walruses kill small whales, pinnipeds always kill and eat animals that are much smaller than themselves. Prey size determines how Alaska fur seals, Steller sea lions, and harbor seals eat their food. Small fishes less than 30 cm long, such as anchovy, herring, saury, or squid, are consumed whole under water (Niggol, Fiscus, and Wilke, 1959; Spaulding, 1964). Except for very small food items like lanternfish, the p�ey is swallowed head first. Larger prey measuring more than 30 cm long, such as hake, rockfish, or salmon, is brought to the surface, grasped by the head, shaken vio- 260 Social Behavior In Some Carnivores lently, and reduced to chunks that are swallowed piecemeal. As fur seals get older and larger, large fish up to proportion of their diet (Spaulding, 92 cm make up an increasingly high 1964). Some pinnipeds feed in groups, but, unlike the case with canids, grouping does not enable them to exploit larger animals. Rather, group ing seems to make it easier for them to exploit large aggregations of small prey. Fiscus and Baines (1966) saw Steller sea lions leaving their hauling grounds in the vicinity of Unimak Pass, Alaska, in compact groups of several hundred to several thousand animals. They swam out to feeding areas, where they dispersed into smaller groups of less than 50 animals containing both sexes and mixed sizes of animals. Massings of this type fed on large schools of fish or squid. Casual observations suggest that groups of sea lions feed more effiCiently on schools of fish because cooperation among the hunters enables them to herd and con trol the movements of the school. When large fish schools are absent, sea lions feed singly or in small groups of two to five animals. Similar behavior has been observed in the California sea lion (Fiscus and Baines, 1966; Michael Bonnell, personal observation). Before Steller sea lion females leave the rookery and their pups to go to sea and feed, they may engage in activities that resemble, and that may have an analogous function to, the prehunt greeting ceremony of wild dogs (Estes and Goddard, Lawick-Goodall, 1967; van Lawick-Goodall and van 1971). Several females may gather at water's edge, where they mill about restlessly in close contact with each other. They may vocalize repeatedly and engage in what appears to be low-intensity aggressive behavior before swimming off as a group to feed. In both species, these activities may reinforce group cohesion and unity as well as synchronize the time of departure. Similarly, canids and pinnipeds may travel several kilometers to get a meal. Van Lawick-Goodall and van Lawick-Goodall (1971) report that wild dogs did not start their first chase until they were 8 km from where they started. The chase itself covered a distance of pursue moose or caribou for up to (Mech, 5 51/2 km. Wolves may to 8 km but usually give up sooner 1970). They may range up to 32 km from their den (Kelsall, 1957). Steller sea lions and California sea lions travel much further from their rookeries or hauling grounds to feed. The former have been seen feed ing as far as 112 to 136 km from land (Kenyon and Rice, 1961) and the 62 km from land (Fiscus and Baines, 1966; Le Boeuf, unpublished data). Usually, only small groups of less than 30 individu latter as far as als are seen this far from land. Large groups of sea lions (100 or more) are seldom seen feeding more than 16 to 24 km from a hauling ground or rookery. It would be interesting to compare land carnivores and pin- Prey, Food Habits, and Feedi ng Behavior 261 nipeds on the s u ccess of thei r respecti v e h u n ti ng a n d fi shing expedi tion s , but the di fficu l ti es i nherent in observ i n g sea l s feed i n g i n the wa ter makes this impos sible at presen t . H owever, some indirect evi d ence is p erti n e n t . Virtu a l l y all fur seals collected early in the day by the U. S . Fish a nd Wild l i fe S ervice had food i n their stomachs (Niggol , Fiscu s , a n d Wi lke, 1959). Al though both p i n n i peds a n d the la rger land carnivores a re noma d i c , the former travel grea ter d i s ta n ces i n t h e i r a n nual feed i n g migra tions . Th e record holder is the Ala ska fur sea l , whi ch migra tes from i ts breed ing gro u n d s i n the Beri n g Sea to i ts wi n ter feeding grou nds along the shores' of the ea s tern a n d western Paci fiC a s far sou th as San Diego, California, and H okkai do, Ja pan , a dista nce of some 5000 km . · Wolves may follow caribou on their a n nual migra tions (Kelsall, 1968). Wi ld dogs a n d d h ol e s , Ceron a/pinus, prefer to h u n t in early morning or early afternoon or ev e n i n g , a l though hu nts occur on moonlight nights a s well (Estes a n d God d ar d , 1967; v,a n La w i c k - G o oda l l a n d v a n Lawick-G ooda ll, 1971; Dav i dar, 1975). Wolves a nd bears may h u n t by day or nigh t . Most of th e smal f er foxes are nocturna l feeders (Bekoff, 1975). Fur seals a n d sea l i o n s are pri mari l y n ig h t a nd early morning feeders. S tomach con te n ts of animals col lected a t vari ous hours a fter su nri se show decrea sing a m o u n ts of food. However, in some a rea s wh ere large schools of fi sh are pres e n t , daytime feedi ng also occurs . Typically, the midday hours are s p e n t resti ng, and feeding a c ti v i ties gradually resume in late afte�noon (Niggol , Fiscu s , and Wilke, 1959; Spaulding, 1964). Th e harbor seal is a daytime feeder ( S p a u l ding, 1964). Ca nids ea t grea t a m ou n ts of food in a single feeding bou t . A wolf may gorge 9 kg of mea t i n a single meal; th i s may represent 20% of its body weigh t . H owever, 'e stima tes of a li ttle more tha n half tha t amou n t per day per wolf may be more representa tive (Mech , 1970). Estes a nd Go.d dard (1967) estima te that wild d ogs average 2.72 kg of mea t per day, or approxima tely 6.7% of their body weigh t . But these ca nids may have to ' go wi thou t eating for several days a t a time . Fi ve to 7 day fa s ts have been recorded i n wolves, and one wolf went for 17 days with o u t food (Mech , 1970). It is common knowledge that domestic dogs ca n go for several days w i thou t ea ting, pa rticu l a rl y when s ex u a l activi ty is probable . G ri zzlies a n d black bears in the more northerly parts of N<;>rth America may go dorma nt for varyi ng periods of time during the win ter when food is scarce. I n ge neral, all of ,the land carnivores will feed regularly if possibl e . Even bears will a ctively feed through ou t the year when food is available. Most pinnipeds feed daily except a t certa i n times of the year, u s u ally the breedi ng season, when they u ndergo long fasts . Th u s, feas ts and 262 Social Behavior In Some Carnivores fa mine are even m ore extreme in their annual cycle. Wh en they are feedi ng, fu r sea l s and sea lions i ngest less food relative to body weight than the ca nids menti oned above. Spaulding (1964) estima tes tha t fur sea l , sea lion, a nd ha rbor seal food requi rements ra nge from 2 to 11 % of body weight per day, with 6% being th e average. During th e breeding season, fu r seal a nd sea lion males fa s t for approxima tely 6 weeks; har bor sea l s do not a p pear to fa st duri ng th i s time. The record for long fa sts i n pinni peds goes to male north ern elepha n t seals. They may go wi th out food for 3 mon ths (Le Boeu f, a nd Peterson 1969). Throughout this time they are actively fig h ti ng and a ttem p ti ng to copula te, and they may lose over 450 kg. Females of this species fas t for an a verage of 34 days during the peri od in whi ch they give bi rth a nd nurse th eir pups daily for 28 days (Le Boeu f, W h i ting, a n d G a ntt, 1972). A female's total body weight may be redu ced by almost 50% during this fast. During the nonbreeding season, males and females fa st aga i n for approxima tely 30 days while un dergoing the a n nual mol t. Most ca nids a n d ursids scavenge for food at some time. Perhaps bears exploi t carrion more tha n any other l a n d ca rnivore. Carri on was a major part of the Ca l i fornia grizzly's diet more than 100 years ago, before man killed th e grizzly and i ts prey. It is said tha t these bears kept th e Califor nia coa s t clea n of pinniped and whale carcasses tha t repea tedly washed ash ore. There are n u m erou s reports of 12 to 15 bears feedi ng a t the same time on a s ingle whale ca rca ss (S torer a n d Tev i s , 1955). Many of the larger land carnivores typically desert the rema i n s of a kill and ca che i t a w a y a n d return to ea t from i t la ter. Polar bears a r e a n excep tion to this rule. They do not cache ringed seal carcasses th a t they kill, despite the fa ct tha t they may only feed on the blubber a n d leave the meat behind ( S ti rl i n g and McEwa n, 1975). In marked contra s t to these feeding habits of la nd carnivores , pinnipeds do not cache food, nor d o they s cavenge, although the walru s may be an excep tion to this rule. Pinnipeds con s u m e most of their prey en tire and discard only th e heads of large fish, particularly rockfish. One drawback that pinnipeds experience i n their marine habitat to which the larger land carnivores are rela tively i mmune is predation. In carving their niche in the sea, pinnipeds beca m e exposed to large pre da tors who were also adapted to aquati c l i fe a n d more formidable than themselves. A t sea, the main predators on pinnipeds are large sharks, particu larly the great white shark, whale, Carcharodon carcharias, and the killer Orcinus orca. All p i n niped speci e s living i n the Pacific are preyed on by these animal s . The degree of preda tion i s u nknown. When pin nipeds return to land or ice to give birth and nurse their young, they are vulnerable to preda tion by large land carnivores such a s polar bears and 263 Socia l Behavior and Reproduction grizzly bea rs, perhaps wolves, a n d, of course, man. S m a l l carnivores, such a s foxes, and skuas, gulls, and ha wks, may prey on their you ng. Being a d a p ted prima rily for locomotion i n wa ter, they are no ma tch defendi ng themselves against la rge land preda tors. Thu s, it woul d ap pear that preda tor pressure wa s importa n t i n ca using pinnipeds to breed in remote sanctu a ries free from these preda tors; tha t is, offshore islands or rocks, sa ndbars a n d ice floes, a n d offshore breeding a reas are limi ted . This is parti cularly tru e of the food-rich wa ters of the temperate north Padfic, the north A tla n ti c, and the circu mpolar A n ta rcti c . 4 SOCIAL BEHAVIOR AND REPRODUCTION Given this set of circum s ta nces, it is li kely that pin nipeds were forced to share limited breeding areas, a n d this led to the high degree of sodality a n d the l a rge soda I gatherings that we fi n d i n pinnipeds toda y . In terms of the sheer number of a nimals tha t congrega te together, the pinnipeds are much more social th an any land ca rnivore. As many a s 2 million Alaska fur seals ca n be fou nd every summer on two tiny Pribilof Islands in the Beri ng Sea . Three thou s a n d northern elepha n t seals may be found packed together tigh tly on one beach on Isla d e Guadalupe, Mexico, during the peak of the breeding s e a s o n (Fig . 9. 2). Two thousand California sea lions may be found sleeping together in close contact 1/2 km inland on S a n Miguel Isla nd, California, during the nonbreeding season ( Fig. 9. 3). The largest wolf or wi ld dog packs are miniscule in comparison. Pi nnipeds are a mong the most p olygynous mammal s . Apparently, this mode of life developed v ery early i n their his tory, and i t was closely tied to their amphibious habi ts (Bartholom ew, 1970). When females bega n clustering i n time and spa ce, the condi tions became ideal for promoting male- male competi ti on, polygyny, and sex ual dimorphism (see Trivers, 1972). Males must have competed to mate with a s many females a s possible. Those who kept other males away from areas con ' taining females sired more p up s . In their s hort breedi ng seasons on tra ditional rookeries, fur seals and sea lions devel oped a s ocial s tructure 9.4 and 9.5). Males chara cterized by territoriality among males (Fig . withou t territories d o not copula te, or they copulate only rarely; those who secure the most well-placed territories-territories containing the most estrou s females--do most of the breeding (Bartholomew, G entry, 1953; 1970). Another syste m that developed was for males to domi na te other males a n d thus gain access to more estrous f emales wherever they were situate d . This was the s tra tegy adopteq by northern elephant ! FIGURE 9.2 Approximately 2000 northern elephant seal males. females. and newborn pups gathered on Pilot Rock Beach. Isla de Guadalupe. Mexico on 5 February 1973. a week after the peak of the breeding season. An observer is located in the extreme foreground. 8i -; ', . 'I" . . ,:",�, "i.�., .. ,,' _ .. ,,-;JV ' . ,: �. .' � J '�'&'= ... �. 'I • � ..., .. . . � ,-.r.. ..� :<l�- �:��?j.:.::. :. :}���: .. .� �. : : : .",',>..:•• -' .� > .::: " ...... .,. ""-....: . ;1", �.; .' .. ��":<:., ..." . A large aggregation of California sea lions about 1/2 km inland from the water's edge on San Miguel Island, California, during the ..0; . . """"" , startled and rose up on their foreflippers. nonbreeding season. Normally the sea lions would be sleeping in close contact with each other, but shortly before the picture was taken they were FIGURE 9.3 • < �.�:>r�/":·�, , ,t,� ,'1 ", �,)"; . .,:�:. ':.;�1,>�' :: 70.'2; ; - , - '�': .,;!_. I .-< , . . .. Both California sea lion males and Alaska fur seal males are territorial during the breeding season, and on San Miguel r' r'";' ... -.�=?:- :.:�. ��:-- �':"'�;;fYmr.� �,. ;..- are sleeping subadult male elephant seals. fluctuations in several variables. The sea lions are all other animals except those circled and those indicated by arrows; the latter foreground) are well defined spatially, and males actively herd females. California sea lions' territories are plastic and change with Island they breed at the same time, and there is often interspecific competition over territories. The fur seal territories (circled in the RGURE 9.4 ... � \-4 -:--...- .-.-.......:.:....-.... - �-� � - Social Behavior and Reproduction FIGURE 9.5 267 Two Steller sea lion males engaged in a mutual threat at the boundary between their respective territories. seal males. They fight for social status in a dominance hierarchy. The highest ranking male or males--depending on the number of females present-locate themselves nearest the females and keep all others away. One or a few of the highest-ranking males do most of the breed ing in a harem; most males in a colony do not breed at all. One male may dominate a harem for several years and inseminate more than 200 females (Le Boeuf, 1974). Grey seal society is similar to that of elephant seals. Thus, in most of the well-studied pinnipeds, males are polygynous and are either territorial or exhibit a dominance hierarchy during the breeding season. This is a reflection of the manner in which the largest and strongest males go about monopolizing a large number of females. Since size and physical aggression convey such a great reproductive advantage to males, these traits have become increasingly elaborated in time. Sexual dimorphism is greater in some seals and sea lions than in other mammals. For example, the Alaska fur seal male is five times larger than the female (Fig. 9.6). Social Behavior In Some Carnivores 268 FIGURE 9.6 Northern fur seal harem on Kitovi Rookery, Pribilof Islands, Alaska, during the breeding season. The male in the foreground has eight females in his territory; the male immediately behind him has none. Note the extreme sexual dimorphism. Photographed by R. S. Peterson in June, 1963. Canids are "social" i n quite a differen t way from pinniped s . Soci a l orga niza tion is chara cterized by seasonal or permanent p a i r b o n d s i n foxes ( S torm, 1965; MacPherson, 1969) a n d jackals ( e. g. , v a n Lawick 1971; Eisenberg and Lockhard, 1972) G oodall and van La wick- G oodall, a n d by coopera tive h u n ting a n d group l i fe in the wolf, d hole, a n d wild dog (Kleiman and Eisenberg, 1973; Mech , 1975). Black bears are essen tially solitary, but female-cub a ssoci a tions are long lasti ng, a n d strong and female cubs i n h erit their mother's feedi n g territories (Rogers, 1974). Wolf packs are orga nized i n to separate male a n d female social hierar chies . Al though the female h ierarch y is more a mbiguous, it is clearly m ore pronounced than the size-related dom i nance observed i n female elephant seals. Hu n ti ng dog pa cks have similar h ierarchies, particularly 269 Social Behavior and Reproduction during the breeding sea s o n , b u t it is the fem a l e h ierarch y tha t is most evident (van Lawick - G ooda l l a n d v a n Lawick- G ooda l l , 1971). I n wolves and wild dogs , the domina n t breeding fem a l e may prev ent s ubordin a te females from breeding or may drive them ou t of th e pack a n d kill their p u p s . Domina nt fe m a l es may also enlist the help of males , and they may prevent other a d u l ts fro m feeding the s u bordina te fem a l e or her you n g . A l though m a n y ca nids a n d ursids a re territoria l i n t h e sense tha t they defend a feeding or h ome ra nge against other individ u a l s or gro u p s of � individ u a l s , this behavior is q u ite different from the defense of rigid individ u a l territories by fu r seal males a n d S teller sea lion males agains t conspecific males d u ring t h e breeding s ea s o n . Wh en i t comes t o repro d u ctiv e behav ior a n d parenta l c a r e , pinnipeds a nd terrestria l carnivores differ in m a n y ways . Most ca nids and u rsids , like pinnipeds, h a v e o n e es tru s per year ( A s d e l l , 1964; Kleim a n , 1968). H owever, male-female sex u a l in tera cti ons in the land ca rniv ores ca n be prolonged a nd complex . Ca nids exhibit extensive preco p u l a tory court ship, frequent olfactory exa mina tion of the partners, a n d frequ en t u rine a nd fa ecal ma rking (Kleima n , 1968) . In some domes tic ca nids, females activ ely solicit m a l e s ( Le Boeu £, 1967) . Ca nid copula tion is characterized by a ge nita l lock or tie . A fter i n tromission is ach ieved, the pair rem a ins j oined together by the genita l ia a nd fa ce in opposite directions for sev eral m i n u tes or a s long a s a n hour. Females may be in es trus for several days a n d may cop u l a te severa l times with d ifferent males . Usually, only th e alpha female in a wolf pack breeds (Kleima n a n d Eisenberg , 1973; Rabb, Wool py, a n d G insburg, 1967; Zimen, 1975, 1976) . I n contrast, th ere is l ittle or no courts h ip in pin niped sexual beh a v ior ( Le Boe u f, 1972) . Male north ern elepha n t sea l s pounce on fema les a n d a tte m p t t o force cop u l a tion a t any t i m e d u ring t h e breeding sea s o n , regardless of t h e fema le's reprodu c tive condition ( Cox a n d Le Boeu£, 1977) . Sea lions a n d fur sea l s may inves tiga te the fema l e ' s genita lia be fore mou nting, b u t there is little parallel to the direct solicita tion of males by females seen in the domestic dog ( B each and Le Boeuf, 1967; Beach , 1976) . Male seals do not mark with urine or feces , a n d they do not exhibit a copula tory lock . Cop u l a tion d u ra tion is equally long in both a nimal grou ps, l a s ting 3 to 7 min in eleph a n t sea ls a n d 15 to 20 min i n S teller sea lions . Some ph ocid females a re in estrus for several days; most otariid females (in which the males are territoria l) cop u l a te only once d u ring the estrou s period ( Le Boeu£, 1972) . Cop u l a tion is m o re ca n em excep t in elep h a n t sea l s , wh ere the male mou n ts from th e rear a n d to one side, perhaps a necessity because of the large bulk of th e animal s . Elep h a n t s e a l males, like male wolves , prevent o r in terr u p t each oth er's cop u la tions; female sea l s , u nlike w o l f bitch es, do not prevent other females 270 Social Behavior In Some Carnivores from ma ting, but they threa ten, attack, and on occa sion ki l l p u p s of neigh bori ng females (Le Boe u f a n d Briggs, Canids have a 1977) . short gesta tion period o f about 2 months a n d a l itter size tha t ra nges from 2 to 15, with 5 being the mode (Asdell, 1964; 1968) . The gesta ti on peri od of bears is 6 to 7% month s , a n d the li tter size is usually 2 to 4 ( A sdell, 1964) . Wild canid p u p s are born in dens or bu rrows. Wea ning from moth er's milk is at abou t 40 days in the Kleima n , domestic dog. B u t d u ring the tra nsition to solid food in most canids, the a l tricial young are provisioned by regurgi ta tion feedi n g of partially mas ticated food . Thi s is done by the pare n t male as well a s by the mother in the smaller pair-bonding ca nids. I n wolves a n d wild dogs, a l l adults in the pack, male a n d fema le, regurgitate to young pups a s well a s to the nursing mother not i nvolved in the h u n t. It is not clear how long this type of provisioni ng of the young goes o n , b u t apparen tly it lasts u n til the you ng are able to obta i n food on their own (see Mech , reviews in Fox, 1970; several 1975) . Females of all pi nniped species give birth to a single pup o n ce a year. They do not eat the placenta as the carnivores d o . With i n a few days or weeks a fter parturi tion, copula tion takes pla ce. G esta tion, i n cluding de layed implanta ti o n , lasts 1 year. Thu s , females give birth a t about the same time each year and u sually in the same place (except for sea l s tha t breed on i ce). Most species give birth i n the open, whether it is on sand, rock or i ce; the ri nged seal is a n exce p ti o n and gives birth i n subnivean lairs in the pressure ri dges of sea ice ( S tirling and McEwan , 1975). New borns of all species a re p h y sically well developed a t birth. Pups are nursed for a minimum of 3 weeks (southern elephant seal, Law s , 1956) 1970; walrus, Brookes, to more tha n a year (Steller sea lion, Gentry , 1954). The northern eleph a n t seal female is a mammalia n phenomenon i n that she doesn't feed during the e n tire 4 week n ursing period! Wea n i n g is abrupt i n s pecies l i k e the elepha n t s e a l a n d gradual i n others like the Pacifi c sea lions. Apparently, mothers feed their pups excl usively by nursing; they have not been observ ed carrying food to the young or regurgita ting to them. Females of several species tha t breed colonially wil l n o t nurse pups other tha n their own. Northern elephan t seal females bite a n d som etimes kill orp h a ned pups that attempt to suckle them. Adoption of orphaned pups by females who have lost their own pups may occur in eleph a n t seals, grey s ea l s , a n d , rarely , i n S teller sea lions. U n like the ca nids, male pinnipeds that breed in colonies do not help in rearing or feeding the young. S o far as I know, no male seal helps the femal e in any way . Quite the contrary, males are i n a dvertently responsible for a sizeable portion of pup mortality i n elepha n t sea l s a n d Social Behavior and Reproduction 271 grey seals (Le Boeuf, Wh i ti ng , a n d G a ntt, 1972; Le Boeuf a n d Briggs, 1977; Coulson a nd H ickling, 1964). Ev idently, there are fu ndamenta l di fferences i n the social and rep ro ductive behavior o f these two rela ted group s of animals . Two basic differences having to do with the origin of sociality and the rate of reprodu cti ve behavior sta n d ou t . Social behavi or s e e m s to h a v e q u i te a d i fferen t origin i n th ese two j a nimal gro u p s; t h a t is, they m u s t have been exposed to qui te different sel ection p ressu res . In the carnivores, there d eveloped a prem i u m on grou ping together, coopera ti ng in the h u n t, and feeding other pack members, because these behav iors increa sed the efficiency of pred a ti o n (Wilson, 1975). Being s ocia l enha nced su rviva l through the process o f kin selection (Maynard S m i t h , 1964; Hamilton, 1964). On the other hand, pinniped sociality seems to have developed fi rst because of a paucity o f breeding s i tes free of preda tors; la ter, t h i s trend becam e elaborated through the opera tion of sexual selection . It is unique to pin nipeds that feed ing a n d breeding have a lways been spatially separa ted and have occurred in di ffere n t habitats (Bartholomew, 1970). Unlike the typical terrestrial carnivore with a n extensive home ra nge i n w h ich mating also ta kes place, p i n n i peds opted to tra vel grea t dista nces to breed on tradi tional sa nctuaries . Since such sanctuaries were limited, females were forced to c l u m p together; this situation predisposed males to maximize their reproductive success by a ttem p ti n g to inseminate as many females as possible ( see Trivers, 1972). S u ccessful males were belligerent and big . Unbridled male-male competition led q u ickly to extreme polygyny, sex ual dimorp h i s m , lack o f parental beha vior on the part of the male, sex differences in beha vior, and conti n ued gathering of a large number of animals for bree d i n g . Once this system was started, the inertia of sexual selection kept it moving in the sam «;; di rection . For example, females that move from the main aggregation o f fema les are apt to be fertilized by a male who is u n succes s ful in competing with other males-that is, a " marginal" male . I f he is subordinate because of genetic factors, his progeny will be less succes sful in the next genera tion . Thus, any ten dency o f females to become less gregarious would be selected against and would be self- limiting (McLaren, 1967). G regariousness in females should be positively reinforced a s long a s competition among males- whether for territories or for socia l status-results in the exclusion of some males (Bartholomew, 1970). The second fu nda mental di fference between pinnipeds a n d terres trial carnivores has to do with reproductive turnover. I n general, natural selection has fa vored two contrasting responses to the problems of 272 Socia l Behavior In Some Carnivores popu l a tion re p l a cement a n d the col onization of new h a bita ts . According to M a cArth ur and W i l son (1 967) , in e n v i ro n m e n ts where there i s li ttle crow d i n g a n d food is abu n d a n t, r - se l ection will fa vor a n imals tha t u ti l ize the most food and rea r the l a rger fa m i l ies-th a t is, those with a high popu l a ti o n g rowth ra te . O n the other h a n d , when there is crowding a n d i n tense com peti tion for food , K - selection w i l l fa vor a ni m a l s t h a t can rep l a ce themselves w i th the lowest possible i n ta ke of food . The fi rst is a stra tegy of p rod uctivity, a n d the seco n d is one of e ffici ency of reproduc tion . In genera l , p i n n i peds a p pear to have been K - selected and terrestrial ca rni vores r - selected-Pi a n ka ' s (1970) a d m o n i t i o n that all orga nisms represent a com p rom i se on t h i s conti n u u m is certa i n l y a pplicable here . One of t h e most i n teresti n g correl a tes of r - a nd K - selection is the pair of com p l exes that M a rtin (1 975) ca l l s , respectively, the a l tricial a n d preco cial com p l ex . Pi n n i peds con form to m a n y of the a ttribu tes of the p reco cia l complex . O n l y one p u p is born per year (one every 2 years in the walrus, a ccord i n g to Brooks, 1 954) a n d the y o u n g exhibit adva nced physical d evelo p m e n t at birth . A l a s ka fu r sea l s ca n locomote and voc alize within a m o m e n t o f birth , and they a re a s adva nced a t birth as d om e s tic dogs a b o u t 3 w e e k s o l d ( B a rt h o l o m e w , 1 959) . Northern elepha nt seal pups open their eyes a n d may exhibit the characteristic sa nd - fli pping movements of the fore fl i p pers before com pletely expelled from the birth ca n a l . Harbor seal pups a re able to swim a n d dive at birth (Bishop, 1967) . Lacta tion i s of long dura tion, lasting more than a year in some sea lions a n d the walru s . Even i n sea l s having a relatively short n u rsing period, energy i n vestment in the offspring may be enormou s . Elepha n t seal m i l k is a p p roxi m a tely 55% fa t, a n d a l though p u p s are wea ned a t 4 weeks of age, they a re left with a tremendous fa t s tore (Fig. 9 . 7) . These pups weigh more a t wea n i n g than they do a year later! Other a ttribu tes of the precoci a l com p l ex are la te sexu a l maturi ty and a long l i fe . Most pinniped females o v u l a te for the first ti m e a t about 2 to 5 years of age; m a les reach sexu a l maturi ty at about 3 to 6 years of age (e . g . , harbor seals, Bigg, 1969; Ala ska fur sea l s , B artholomew a n d Hoe!, 1953; anonymous, 1 975; northern eleph a n t seals, Le Boeu£, u npublished data ) . I n the colonial species, males d o not complete s u ccessfully for females u n til much later i n l i fe, u n ti l approximately 10+ years of age in the northern eleph a n t seal (Le Boeuf, 1974) . Pinnipeds may live a long time . Harbor sea ls live for 20 to 29 years (B iggs, 1969) . Alaska fur seals for up to 26 years (Niggo!, Fiscu s, and Wilke, 1959) , and the life span o f the northern eleph a n t seal is typical l y abou t 1 4 years (Le Boeuf a n d Briggs, unpublished data ) . Like that in pri mates a nd other long- lived mammals, Social Behavior and Reproduction FIGURE 9.7 273 A suckling northern elep hant seal pup appears almost as large as its mother shortly before it is weaned. At this time, it is fou r or five times larger than it was at birth, and heavier than it will be a year later. Its sole nurturance has come from mother's milk. The mother has provided this enormous fat store to her offspring during a four-week period in which she did not feed. the trend i n pinni ped repro d u ction h a s been toward q u a l i ty ra ther than quantity. Terres tri a l carni vores, especially the s m a l l e r ca n i d s , exhibit the a l tri cial complex . Most can ids give birth in a shelter to a la rge li tter of p u p s t h a t a re poorly developed a n d i nca pable of coordinated behavior except suckli n g . G esta tion i s short a n d lactation brief co mpared to most p i n niped s . S e x u a l ma tu rity occu rs a t a n early a g e , a t t h e end o f the second year in wild wolves (Mech , 1970) , and a t the end of the fI rst year i s some captive wolves ( Medj o and Mech , 1976) a n d i n the A rctic fox (Chese more, 1975) . L i fe spans a re s h ort compared to those o f pinnipeds . Al though wolves have lived 10 to 16 years i n ca ptivity (You n g , 1944), 10 is considered very old (Mech, 1970) . Seven is considered old age i n l arge dogs (Fuller and D u B u i s , 1962) . The l i fe spa n of faxes is considerably less, 6 years of age i n the gray fox, Urocyon littoralis (Laughlin, 1973). Apparently, ca n i d s and ursids h a v e been selected for q u a n ti ty i n repro duction . The paradoxically low fecu n d i ty rate i n wolf and wild dog packs, wh ere only one female may give birth , may be relate d to low availabi l i ty of food . It i s i n teresting to note that i f only one female gives birth i n a pack, i t is usually the most domina n t one. One might expect ' the fecun d i ty rate to go up when food was abunda n t . Whether this 274 Social Behavior In Some Carnivores happens or not, the point is tha t the potential for a h i g h reprod uctive ra te i s there . U n l i ke l a n d ca rn i vores, pi n n i peds m ll s t d e a l with problem s t h a t arise from maki ng a living to two quite di fferent habitats. The high metabolic rate a nd th i ck layer of b l u bber that a re so adapti ve in cold water crea te d i ffi c u l ties in thermo reg u l a ti o n on l a n d , pa rticu l a rly when the ambient temperature i s h i g h , or a fte r s ll sta i n e d ac tivi ty has taken place . N u mer ous behavioral adj u stments are importa n t in preventing overheating ( W h i te a nd Odel l , 1 971 ; G en try, 1 973; Odell, 1 974; Whittow, 1 974 ) . In a d d i tion, one negative conseq uence o f the la rge social gatherings on land i s that the a ni ma l s a re more v u l n erable to disease a n d parasite tra n s mi ssion . The colon i a l breeding sea l i on s contain a wide variety of i n ternal a n d externa l pa rasi tes ( D a i l ey and B rownell, 1 972) . For exa mple, seven s pecies o f trematod es, eigh t s peci es o f n e m a todes, one cestode, and variou s speci es of Aca n th ocep h a l a , Aca ri n a , a n d Anoplura have been identi fied i n the Ca li fornia sea l i on . L u n gworm i n festa tion is par ticu larly heavy i n this speci es and may be a n i m porta n t ca u se of j u venile morta l i ty . La n d c a rn ivores do not seem to harbor the variety or the same high levels of para s i te i n festation as do these ma rine mamma l s . H ow ever, this comparison needs to be i n vestiga ted m ore systema tica l l y . 5 S UMMARY I n s u m mary, these two groups of a ni m a l s have obvious simila ri ties , some of which ca n be a ttribu ted to h a v i n g a common ancestor . Pin nipeds bear the stamp of their ca nine, u rsine, a n d lu trine rela tives, especi a l l y i n th e stru ctu re of the head and the pronou nced ca nine teeth . Most species in both orders are flesh ea ters; they ea t grea t qua n ti ties a t a ti me, a n d they ea t a wi de variety of prey . However, subsistence in different h abitats a n d exp o s u re to differenti a l selection for a t least 20 million years have produ ced some even more obvious a n d interesting differences in social behavi'o r. The larger canids evidently were selected to join social group s , in which there is a high degree of coop eration, because this allowed them to exploi t la rge prey . Canids tha t prey on a nimals smaller t h a n themselves are far less s ocial , and kin- n e twork welfare is red u ced . Pi nnipeds are social for a differen t reason . The most gregari ous of them congrega te i n immense gath eri ngs tha t may include thousa nds of i ndividuals . These aggrega tions are l oca ted o n offshore areas t h a t are free from preda tors . Si nce these l oca tions are restri cted in number a nd size, it appears tha t p i n nipeds were forced to group to gether in order to avoid preda tors . Once this s tep was taken, sexual 275 References selecti on p u t a premi u m on conti n u ed soci a l l i v i ng; for exa mple, males who d om i na ted oth er males i n th e l a rge social g a therings probably bore mo re offspring th a n those who pair- bonded w i th s ingle females a way from th e grou p . The few p i n n i ped s p ecies tha t are soli tary live in a habitat wh ere there a re few or no preda tors a n d wh ere hauling-out a reas, such a s pack ice, are vi rtu a l l y u nl i m i te d . Fina l l y , s tra tegies of reprod u ction seem to d i ffer i n the two groups of a n i mal s . I n genera l , the terres tri a l ca rni vores a p pea r to have been selected to produce a grea t quanti ty of offspring q u i ckly, a stra tegy tha t works best in a varying envi ron ment , wh i l e p i n n i p ed s a ppea r to have been selected to produce fewer p rogeny of h igh q u a l i ty , a stra tegy that is most characteri stic of s table envi ron ments . REFERENCES S. S . , W. N. B o n n e r , j . R. B a k e r, a n d R. Rich a r d s . 1974 . G rey seals, Halichoems grYl'lIs, of the Dee E s t u a ry a n d observa t i o n s o n a ch a racteristi c skin lesion in British se al s . j. Zool . ( Lond . ) 1 74 : 429-440. A n derso n , S . A . 1 964 . Pattems of Mallllllaliml Rel'rodu ctioll . 2nd ed . Cornell University Press, New York. 670 p. Asd e l l , Ba rtholomew, G. A . 1 953. Behavioral factors affecting social structure in the Alaska fur sea l . Trans. 1 8th No. Amer. Wildl . Con f. 481-502 . Barth olomew, G . A . 1959 . Mother-young rela tions and the matu ra tion of pup behavior in the Alaska fur sea l . Ani m . Beha v . 7: 163-- 171 . Bartholomew, G . A . 1970. A model for the evol u tion of pin niped polygyny. Evolu tion 24 : 546-559 . Bartholomew, G . A . and P. G . Hoel. 1953 . Reproductive behavi our of the Alaska fur seal, Callorh illus u rsill u s . j. M a m m . 34 : 417-436. Beach , F . A. 1976. Sexual attractivity, procep tivity, and receptivity i n female mammals. Horm . a n d Beha v . 7: 105-138 . Beach, F. A. and B. J . Le Boeuf. 1967. Coital behavior in dogs: I. Preferential mating in the bitch . Anim. Beha v . 15: 546-558 . Bekoff, M . 1975. Social behavior and ecology of the A frica n Canidae: A review. III: M . W. Fox, Ed. The Wild Canids . Van Nostrand Rei nhold, N e w York. P . 120-142. Bigg, M . A . 1969. The harbour seal i n B ritish Colu mbia . Fisheries Research Board of Canada Bulleti n , Otta w a . 33 p . Bishop, R . H . 1967. Reproduction, age determi nation , a n d behavior o f t h e harbor seal, Phoca vitu lina L . , in the Gulf of Alaska . U n published M. S . thesis, University of Alaska, 121 p. Bonnell, M. L. and R . K . Selander. 1974 . Elephant seals: genetic variation a nd near extinc tion. Science 184: 908--909. Brooks, J. W. 1954. A contribution to the life history and ecology of the Pacific walru s . Unpublished M . S . thesis, U niversity o f Alaska . 103 p . 276 Social Behavior In Some Carnivores Bruce, W. S. 1915. M easu rements a nd weigh ts of a n tarcti c sea ls, Part I I . [II: R eport Oil ti,e Scit'll tijic R , 'slIlls of ti, e VOYllge or s. Y. 'Scolia ' dll rillg ti, e yenrs 1902 , 1903 , alld 1904 . Scotti sh Ocea n ographic Labora tory, Ed i n b u rgh . P. ]49-]74 . Chesl'morl', D. L. 1975. Ecology of the Arcti c fox (A lopex IlIgOp I I S ) in North A merica : a review. I I I : M . W . Fox, Ed . Ti, e Wild Cll / l ids . V a n Nos tra n d Rei nhold , New York . P. 143-163. Cou lson, j. C . and G. H i ckl i ng . 1964 . TIll' breed i ng biology of the grey sea l , Halichoerus g ryplls (Fa b . ) on the Farnl' I s l a n d s , North u m berl a n d . j. A n i m . Ecol . 33 : 485-512 . Cox, C. R. a nd B. j . Le BOl'uf. 1 977. Female i n ci ta tion of male competition: A mechanism in sexu a l selectio n . A m . Nat. 1 1 1 : 31 7-335. Dailey, M . D. and R . L. Brownell, J r . 1972 . A checkl ist of marine mammal parasi tes . ill: S. H . Ridgeway, Ed . Mam milis of the Sell: Biology al/d Medicille. Charles C . Thomas, S p ri n gfiel d , I I I . P . 528-576 . Davidar, E. R. C. 1975. Ecology a n d behavior of the Dhole or I n d i a n wild dog, CIIOII a/pinus ( Pallas) . I I I : M . W . Fox, Ed . Th e Wild Cal / ids . Van Nostrand Rei nhold, New York . P . 1 09--119. Davies, j . L. 1 958a . The Pinni pedia: a n essay i n zoogeogra phy. G eogr. Rev. 48: 474-493 . Davies, j . L. 1 958b. Pleistocene gl'ogr<1 p h y <1 n d the distribu tion of northern pi n ni peds . Ecology 39: 97-1 13. Downs, T . ]956. A new pinni ped from the Mi ocene of southern Cali fornia : wi th remarks on the Otariidae. j. Paleontol . 30: 1 1 5-131. Eisenberg, j . F . and M. Lockh art. 1 972 . A n ecologi cal reconnaissa n ce of Wilpa ttu National Park. Smithson . Contrib. Knowl. (Zool . ) . 101: 1-118 . Erdbri nk, D. P. 1953 . A RI''1'il7l) of Fossil alld Recel / I Bears of th e Old World. 2 parts . Deventer, De Lange. 597 p . Estes, R. D . <1 n d j . Goddard . 1967. Prey selecti on a n d h u nting behavior of t h e Africa n wild dog. j . Wildlife Manag. 31: 52-70. Fiscus, C . H . and G. A. Baines. ]966 . Food a n d feeding behavior of S teller and Cali fornia sea lions. j. M a m m . 47: 195-200 . Fiscu s , C. H . , K. Niggol, and F. Wilke. ]%1 . Pelagic Fur Seal I nvestigations: Cali fornia to British Col u mbia . U . S . Fish a nd Wildlife Service, Seattle, Washi ngton . 87 p . Fox, M . W . , Ed . 1975 . TI,,' Wild Cll /l ids . V a n N ostrand Reinhold, N e w York . 508 p . F u ller, j . L . <1 n d E . M . D u B u i s . ]962. T h e behavior of dogs. [ 1 1 : E . H a fez, Ed . Ti, e Bchallior of Domestic A ll imais . Bailliere, Ti ndall, and Cox, London . P . 415-452. G entry, R . L . 1970. S ocial behavior of the Steller sea lion. U n pu blished doctoral dissertation, U niversity of Cali fornia, Santa Cru z . 113 p . Gentry, R . L . 1973 . Thermoregula tory behavior o f earred seals. Behaviour 46: 73-93 . H a milton, W. D. 1964 . The genetical theory of social behaviour. I, I I . j . Th eor. BioI. 7: 1-52. H endley, Q. B. 1972 . The evolu tion and dispersal of the Monoch i na e ( M a m malia: Pinnipedia) . A n n . S . Afr. Mus. 59: 99-113. H uey, L . M. 1930. Capture of a n elephant seal off San Diego, Cali forni a , wi th notes on stomach contents. j. Mamm. 11: 229-231 . IIIiger, C. 1811 . Prodrolll lls Systellla tis Mamma/iu lIl et AviulI I . Salfeld, Berli n . Kelsall, J. P . 1968 . The Migratory Barrell G rou lld Cariboll of Cal/ada . Ca n . Wildl . Serv . , Queen's Pri nter, Ottawa . 340 p . References 277 Kenyon, K. W. 1 965. Food of h� rbor sl'�ls at A mchitk� I s b n d , Alaska . 103-- lO4 . j. M � m m . 46: Kenyon, K. W. and D. W. Hice. 1 961 . Abu nd� nce � n d distri b u tion of the Steller sea lion . M� m m . 42: 233-- 234 . King, j . E. 1 964 . SClIls or I h e World . j. British M u seu m ( Na t u m l H i s tory) , London . 154 p . Klei m�n, D . C . 1 96H . Heprod u ction i n t h e Ca nidae. I n t. Zoo ! . Yea rb. 8 : 3-8 . Klei m � n , D . G . a n d j . F . Eisen berg . 1973. Comp�risons of c� nid a n d felid soci a l systems from an evol u tion a ry perspl'cti v e . A ni m . Beha v . 2 1 : 637-659. Laughri n , L. L. 1 973 . Ca li fornia I s l a nd fox su rvey . W i ld ! . M'lIla g . Branch A d m . Rept. N o . 73- 3 . 1 7 p . L a w s , R . M . 1 953 . The e l e p h a n t s e a l (Miml/llsn le"" i,la, Li n n . ) . ! . G rowth and a g e . Falkland Is!. Depend . Su r v. Sci . Repts. N o . 8: 1-62 . Laws, R. M . 1956. The elepha nt seal (MirOllllgl1 ICOIlilll1, L i nn . ) . I I . G eneral soci a l and reprod u ctive behavior. Falkland Is!. Depe n d . S u r v . Sci . Repts . 13: 1-88 . Le Boeu f, B . j . 1967. I n teri ndividual associa tions i n d ogs . Behaviour 29: 268-295. Le Boeu f, B . j. 1 9 72. Sexual behavior i n the north ern eleph a n t sea l , M i roungn ang llstiroslris . Behaviour 4 1 : 1-26. Le Boeu f, B. j. 1974 . Male-male com petition a nd reprod u ctive success in eleph a n t sea l s . A m . Zoo! . 14: 1 63-- 1 76 . Le Boe u f, B . Le Boe u f, B . j . 1 977. Back from extation? Paci fi c Di scovery 30: 1-9. j. a nd K. T. Briggs. 1 977. The cost of l i v i ng i n a sea l h a re m . Mamma lia . 41: 1 67-1 95. Le Boeu f, B. j. a nd H . S . Peterson . 1969. Social status a nd mating acti v i t y i n elepha n t sea l s . Science 163: 91-93 . Le Boeu f, B. j . , R. j . Whiting, a nd R. F. G a n tt. 1 972 . Peri natal behavior of northern elep h a n t seal fem a les a nd their young. Beh�viour 43: 121-156 . MacArth ur, R . H . a n d E . O . Wilson . 1 967. Th e Theory of /slalld B iog eog rap hy . Pri nceton University Press, Pri nceton . 203 p . MacPh erson , A . H . 1969 . T h e dyna mics o f Ca n a d i a n Arctic fox pop u l a tions. Ca n . Wild I . Serv o Rept. Ser. 8 , Queen's Pri nter, Otta wa . Marti n , R. D. 1 975. Stra tegies of reprod u cti o n . N � t . H i s t . 84: 48--57. Ma tthew, W. D. 1 939 . Clilllnle a l l d E1'oll l liol l . 2nd ed . Special Publications. New York Academy of Science. Vol 1 . 223 p . M a y nard S m i t h , j. 1964 . Kin selection a n d group selecti o n . N a tu re 201: 1 145-1147. McKenna , M. 1969. The origi n a nd early di fferenti a tion of theria n m a m mals. Ann. N . Y . A ca d . Sci . 167: 217-240. McLare n , I . A. 1960. A re the P i nni pedia biphyl etic? Syst. Zoo! . 9: 18--28 . McLaren, I . A . 1967. Sea ls a n d group selecti o n . Ecology 48: 104-110. M ech , L. D. 1970 . Th e Wolf: Ti, e Ecology and Behavior of an Enda/lgered Species . Natu ral H i s tory Press, G a rden City, New York . 384 p. Mech , L . D. 1975 . H u n ti ng beha vior i n two similar species of social can i d s . In: M . W . Fox, Ed . Til e Wild Ca n ids . Van Nostra nd Rei nhold, New York. P . 363--368 . Medjo, D. C . and L . D . Mech . 1976 . Reprod u ctive acti v i t y in nine- a n d ten-month-old wolves. j. Mamm. 57: 406-408 . Social Behavior In Some Carnlvorea 278 M i tchL'l I , E. a n d R. H . Tedfor d . 1973. The E n a l i a rctinae, 151: Mivart, of ex ti nct aquatic Bull. Am. M u s . Nat. Hist. a new gro u p Carn iv ora a nd a considera t i o n of t h e ori gi n of t h e Otaridal' . 201-284 . G . 1885. N otes on t h e Pi n n i ped i a . Proc. Zoo! . Soc. L on do n : 484-500. M o n ta g n a , W . and R . l'itll /illll ) . A m . M orejoh n , j. j. H a r rison . 1957. SpeCi a l i z a t i o n s in th e skin of the sea l (Phoca A n a t . 100: 81- 1 1 4 . G. V . a n d D . M . B ,l l tz . 1970. C on t e n ts of t h e stomach of a n elephant s eal . 1 73-174 . J. M a m m . 51 : N iggol , K . , C . H . Fi s cu s, a n d F . W i l k e . 1959. Prlagic Fll r Sral lllll('stiga tioll s : Califomia- Oregoll alld Wa�/l illg fcm . U . S . F i s h a n d W i l d l i fe S e r v i c e , Sea ttle, Washi ngton . 92 p . Odel l , D . K . 1974 . B e h a v ior a l th er m oregu l a t i on i n t h e C a l i fornia Sea Lion . Beha v . BioI . 10: 231-237 . Pia nka , E. R. 1970. On r- a nd K - sel ecti on . A m . Nat. 104: 592-597. Rabb, G . B . , j . H . Wool p y , and B . E. G i n s be r g . 1 967. Soci a l rela tionships in a group of ca ptive wolves. A m . Zool . 7 : 305-31 1 . Re p e n n ing, C . A . 1970. P i n n i ped evol u t ion: t h e fossil record . Tal k given a t a symposiu m on m a r i n e biolog y . 21 N ovember 1 970. U n i versity of Ca lifornia, Sa n ta Cru z . Rid g ew a y , S . H . 1972 . H o m eosta s i s i n t h e a qu a ti c e n v i ron m e n t . III: S . H . Ridgewa y , Ed . Mam mals of the Sea . Cha r l e s C. Thomas, S p ri ngfi e l d , I I I . P. 590-747. Rogers, L. L. 1 974 . M o v e m e n t patterns and socia l o r ga n i zatio n of black bears i n M i n nesota . U npublished doctora l disserta tion, U n i ve r s i t y of M i n n esota , M i n neapolis. R u ssel l , R . H. 1975. Th e food habits of polar bea rs of Ja mes Bay a nd south western H u dson Bay i n s u m mer and a u tu m n . A rctic 28 : 1 1 7-129. Sarich, V. M. 1 969. P i n ni p e d ori g i n s and the rate of evol u tion of carnivore albu mi n s . Syst. Zool . 18: 286-295. Sch effer, V. B. 1958 . Seals, Sea Liolls a lld Walruses . S ta nford Uni versity Press, Stanford, Ca l . 1 79 p . Schu sterm a n , R . j . 1972 . Vis u a l i n pi n n i ped s . Ill: H . E. Winn a nd B . L . O l l a , Eds . Vol . 2 . Plen u m Publishing, New York . P. 469-492 . acuity B" hmlior of Marill£' Mammals . S i m p s on , G . G . 1945. The p ri n ci p les of classi fica tion a n d a classifi ca tion of mammals. Bull. A m . M u s . Nat. H i s t . 85. 350 p . Spaulding, D . J . 1964 . Compara tive Feedillg Habits of t h e F u r Seal, Sea LiOIl , alld Harbou r Seal on the Bri tish Colu m bia Coas t . Fish eries Research Board of Canada, Otta wa . 52 p . S tirling, I . a n d E . H . McEwa n . 1975. T h e caloric value of whole ri nged seals (Phoca h isl'ida) in relation to polar bear (Urs u s ma ritill/ lls ) ecology and h u n ting behavior. Ca n . J. Zool . 53: 1021-1027. Storm , G . L. 1965 . Movements and activi ties of foxes as determi ned b y radio-tracking. W i ld l . Manag. 2 9 : 1-13 . J. Trivers, R . L. 1972 . Pa rental i nvestment and sexual selecti o n . In: B. Campbell, Ed. Sexual Selection and the Descen t of Man, 1871-1971 . AldinI', Chicago. P . 136-179. van Lawick- G ooda l l , H . and J. van Lawick- G ood a l l . 1971 . I n n ocen t Killers . Houghton Miffl i n , Boston. 222 p . White, F . N . and D . K . Odel l . 1 971 . Thermoregula tory behavior of the northern elephant sea l , M i roll nga a llgllstiros tris . J. Mamm. 52: 758-774. Whittow, G . C. 1974 . S u n , sa n d , and sea lions. N a t . H i s t . 1974 . 56-63 . References 279 Wilson, E. O. 1975 . Socio/Jiology: Till' N,711 Syn tlresis . Bel k n a p Press , H a rvard U n i versity Press, Cambridgl" Wyma n , j. 1967. You n g , S . Mass. flY7 p. The jackals of t h e Se re n g eti . A n i m a l s 10: 79-83 . P. 1944 . Tire Wol ! ' l's ol Nortll A m erim, P a r t I . A m eri ca n W i l d l i fe I n s ti tu te, Wa s h i n g to n , D . C . 385 p. limen, E . [ 975. So ci a l d y n a m i cs of the wolf pack. Ill: M. W . Fox, Ed . Tir e Wild Cmlids . Van N ostra n d Rei n h o l d , N e w Yor k . P . 336-362 . l i m en , E. 1976. On t h e regu l a t i o n of pack size in wolves. l. Tierpsych ol . 40: 300-34 1 .
© Copyright 2025 Paperzz