Teacher Copy, Level 2 Name_________________ Beetle Battles Featured scientist: Erin McCullough from the University of Western Australia Research Background: Male animals spend a lot of time and energy trying to attract females. In some species, males directly fight with other males to become socially dominant. They also fight to take over and control important territories. This process is known as male-male competition. The large antlers of male elk are an example of a trait that has been favored by male-male competition. In other species, males try to court females directly. This process is known as female choice. The flashy tails of male peacocks are a good example of a trait that has been favored by female choice. Lastly, in some species, both malemale competition AND female choice Erin has always loved beetles! Here she is with a determine which males get to mate. In dung beetle in Tanzania, during a graduate school order to be successful, males have to be class trip. good at both fighting other males and making themselves attractive to females. Erin is a biologist interested in these different types of mating systems. She wondered if she could discover a single trait that was favored by both male-male competition and female choice. In horned dung beetles, male-male competition and female choice are both important in determining which males get to mate. Females dig tunnels underneath fresh piles of dung where they mate and lay their eggs. Beetles only mate inside these underground tunnels, so males fight with other males to become the owner of a tunnel. Males that control the tunnels have a better chance to mate with the female that dug it. However, there is often more than one male inside a breeding tunnel. Small males will sneak inside a main tunnel by digging a connecting side tunnel. Additionally, the constant fights between large males means that the ownership of tunnels is constantly changing. As a result, females meet many different males inside their tunnels. It is up to them to choose the male they find the most attractive, and with whom they’ll mate. In this species of dung beetle, males try to persuade females to mate by quickly tapping on the females’ back with their forelegs and antennae. Previous research has found that females are more likely to mate with males that perform this courtship tapping at a fast rate. Because both fighting and courtship tapping take a lot of strength, Erin wondered if the trait of strength was what she was looking for. Would stronger male dung beetles be favored by both male-male competition and female choice? Data Nuggets developed by Michigan State University fellows in the NSF BEACON and GK-12 programs 1 Teacher Copy, Level 2 Name_________________ To test her hypothesis, Erin conducted a series of experiments to measure the mating success, fighting success, and strength of male dung beetles. First, Erin measured the mating success of male beetles by placing one male and one female in an artificial tunnel (a piece of clear plastic tubing). She watched the pair for one hour, and measured how quickly the males courted, and whether or not the pair mated. Second, Erin measured the fighting Two dung beetle males fighting for ownership of the artificial tunnel. success of males by staging Why is the photo pink? Because beetles mate and fight in dark, fights between two males over underground tunnels, Erin carried out all of her experiments in a dark room under dim red-filtered light. Beetles can’t see the color ownership of an artificial red, so working under red-filtered light didn’t affect the beetles’ tunnel. Beetle battles consist of behavior, and allowed Erin to see what the beetles were doing. a head-to-head pushing match that results in one male getting pushed out of the tunnel, and the other male remaining inside. To analyze the outcome of these fights, Erin randomly selected one male in each pair as the focal male, and scored the interaction as a “win” if the focal male remained inside the tunnel, and as a “loss” if the focal male got pushed out of the tunnel. In some cases, there was not a clear winner and loser because either both males left the tunnel, or both males remained inside. These interactions were scored as a “tie”. Finally, Erin determined each beetles’ strength. She measured strength as the amount of force it took to pull a male out of an artificial tunnel. To do this, she superglued a piece of string to the back of the beetle, had it crawl into an artificial tunnel, attached the string to a spring scale, and then pulled on the scale until the beetle was pulled out of the tunnel. To keep beetles alive in the lab, Erin set up a bucket with sand, and placed one pile of dung in the center (left). Female beetles dug tunnels below the dung (right). Data Nuggets developed by Michigan State University fellows in the NSF BEACON and GK-12 programs 2 Teacher Copy, Level 2 Name_________________ Check for Understanding: After reading the introduction, students should be able to: • Describe the two methods (male-male competition and female choice) that male animals use to try and get mates. How are the two methods different, and how are they alike? • Explain how traits such as a peacock’s tail, or a dung beetle’s strength could change over time due to male-male competition or female choice. • Describe why Erin thought that strength might be a trait favored by male-male competition and female choice. How might strength affect the outcome of fighting for tunnel territories and attracting females? • In their own words, describe the three experiments that Erin ran to test her hypothesis. What were the three variables measured in these experiments? Mating success, fighting success, and strength. Scientific Question: How does strength affect a male horned dung beetles’ chances of winning fights or being chosen as a mate? What is the hypothesis? Find the hypothesis in the Research Background and underline it. A hypothesis is a proposed explanation for an observation, which can then be tested with experimentation or other types of studies. Scientific Data: Use the data from the two tables below to answer the scientific question: Table 1. Strength measurements in millinewtons (mN) for males that did and did not mate with a female. Mated Not mated Strength (mN) 94.9 ± 7.3 113.1 ± 8.7 (mean ± standard error) Table 2. Difference in strength (measured in millinewtons [mN]) between a focal male and his opponent for trials where the focal male won, tied, and lost male-male fights. (A positive value means that the focal male was stronger than his opponent, while a negative value means that the focal male was weaker than his opponent.) Win Tie Loss Difference in strength (mN) 27.9 ± 11.6 -8.0 ± 17.4 -28.7 ± 12.4 (mean ± standard error) * Standard error (SE) tells us how confident we are in our estimate of the mean, and depends on the number of replicates in an experiment and how much variation is in the data. A large SE means we are not very confident, while a small SE means we are more confident. Data Nuggets developed by Michigan State University fellows in the NSF BEACON and GK-12 programs 3 Teacher Copy, Level 2 Name_________________ Check for Understanding: After reading about how the data was calculated and presented, students should be able to: • Identify whether high or low values indicate greater strength in Table 1. • Explain what the positive and negative values in Table 2 tell us. What would a value of “0” mean? How can we use this information to understand how strength affects the outcome of fights? • Summarize how the interpretation of the values in Table 1 is different than in Table 2. Understand that the measure of strength in Table 1 is for a single male, while the measure of strength in Table 2 is the difference between the 2 male opponents. • Describe what you would predict the values to look like if Erin’s hypothesis is true? Teacher Note: You can have students add error bars to their graphs to deepen this discussion or remove SE from the table for younger students. Error bars based on standard deviation (SD) give us information on the amount of variation in the data. They can be used to draw attention to large or small amounts of variation around the mean. Standard error (SE) is the SD divided by the square root of the study’s sample size (SE=SD/√n). Unlike SD, SE reflects the uncertainty in our estimate of the mean. The larger our sample size and the less variation in the data, the more confident we can be in our estimate of the mean. Upper error bars are calculated by adding one SE to the mean, and lower bars are calculated by subtracting one SE from the mean. In the data there is a lot of variation, signified by large error bars. Relative to the signal, the noise is strong. Graph 1: The SE bars do not overlap, but Erin found no statistical difference in strength between males that mated and those that did not. Students will not be able to conclude this from the data directly, but you can have a class discussion about statistical versus biological significance. Do students think a difference of only 18.2 mN is something that female beetles could tell about a male? Would that difference affect the quick back taps males use to attract females? There is not just one answer to this question, but a class discussion might help students decide if this is an important difference in light of the biology of the system. Graph 2: The difference in strength was statistically different between males that won battles and those that lost. The males that tied were not statistically different from either the winning or losing males. This means that the difference in strength between a focal male and his opponent did partially determine which male would win in a beetle battle. What data will you graph from Table 1 to answer the question? Independent variable: Average strength (mN) Dependent variable: Mating success (mated or not mated) What data will you graph from Table 2 to answer the question? Independent variable: Difference in strength (mN) Dependent variable: Fighting success (win, tie, or loss) Data Nuggets developed by Michigan State University fellows in the NSF BEACON and GK-12 programs 4 Name_________________ Teacher Copy, Level 2 Draw your graphs below: Graph 1: Strength (mN) 140 120 100 80 60 40 20 0 Mated Unmated Mating trial outcome Difference in Strength Graph 2: 50 40 30 20 10 0 -10 -20 -30 -40 -50 Win Tie Loss Fighting trial outcome Data Nuggets developed by Michigan State University fellows in the NSF BEACON and GK-12 programs 5 Teacher Copy, Level 2 Name_________________ Interpret the data: Make a claim that answers the scientific question. Strength had no effect on a male’s chances of mating. In contrast, strength had a big effect on a male’s chance of winning fights. If a male was stronger than his opponent, he was more likely to win the fight, and if the male was weaker than his opponent, he was more likely to lose the fight. Teacher Note: It is ok if students conclude that the strength of males who mated with females is different than the strength of males who did not mate. Because students do not have access to the statistics, it is up to them to determine if they feel there is a large enough difference between the bars in Graph 1 to be important biologically. What evidence was used to write your claim? Reference specific parts of the tables or graphs. Strength was very similar for males that did and did not mate. Males that mated had an average strength of 94.9 mN, while males that did not mate had an average strength of 113.1 mN. There is a lot of variation in these data (the standard error bars are almost overlapping), and this difference is not statistically significant. The difference in strength between a male and his opponent was not the same among males that won, tied, or lost fights. Males that won fights had strengths that were on average 27.9 mN higher than their opponents. Males that lost fights, on the other hand, had strengths that were on average 28.7 mN lower than their opponents. Finally, males that tied fights had very similar strengths to their opponents. The average difference in strength among males that tied was only 8 mN. Explain your reasoning and why the evidence supports your claim. Connect the data back to what you learned about the different strategies used by male dung beetles to attract females and fight off males. Females do not prefer to mate with strong males. There was no difference in strength between males that mated versus males that didn’t mate. We know that female dung beetles are attracted to males when they tap their backs, and maybe strength does not come into play in this trait. Maybe it is another aspect of physical performance, such as endurance, that females would find more attractive. The results from this experiment just looked at strength, and suggest that strength does not affect a male’s likelihood of being chosen as a mate. Data Nuggets developed by Michigan State University fellows in the NSF BEACON and GK-12 programs 6 Teacher Copy, Level 2 Name_________________ On the other hand, strength did affect a male’s likelihood of winning fights with other males. In other words, males that won fights were able to resist more force than their opponents before being pulled out their tunnel. Males that were stronger than their opponents were more likely to win fights, and males that were weaker than their opponents were more likely to lose fights. When a male fights another male that is equally strong, neither male won the fight, and the interaction ended in a tie. What does the data from this study tell us about the scientist’s hypothesis? The data do not fully support the hypothesis that strength is favored by both male-male competition and female choice. Strength appears to be favored by male-male competition, because strong males are more likely to win fights. But strength does not appear to be favored by female choice, because strong males are not chosen more often as mates. Your next steps as a scientist: Science is an ongoing process. What new question do you think should be investigated? What future data should be collected to answer this question? See Teacher Note below. Teacher Note: In this study, Erin used strength as a measure of the beetles’ physical performance. This measure of performance is probably very biologically important, because males that are stronger and can resist more force should be better at defending their breeding tunnels. However, strength is only one measure of physical performance. In addition to being strong, it may also be beneficial for beetles to be fast, or to have high endurance. Erin is now interested in looking at how other measures of physical performance (like speed, agility, or endurance) may also affect a male’s likelihood of winning fights and being chosen as a mate. Students can discuss how they would design an experiment to measure speed or endurance in the dung beetles. Students will probably generate a wide diversity of questions for in this system. You can have a class discussion where you jot down all the questions up on the board. Be prepared to ask your students to clarify or justify another student's response in a class discussion. Do students see any ways to improve each other's questions? Are some questions untestable? Remember, if your class wants to send their questions about the study system to Erin, the scientist studying dung beetles, they can email them to datanuggetsk16@gmail.com! Additional teacher resources related to this Data Nugget: A scientific paper published on this research: McCullough, E.L. and L.W. Simmons (2016) Selection on male physical performance during male–male competition and female choice. Behavioral Ecology • http://datanuggets.org/wp-content/uploads/2016/08/McCullough-and-Simmons2016.pdf Data Nuggets developed by Michigan State University fellows in the NSF BEACON and GK-12 programs 7
© Copyright 2025 Paperzz