Reprinted{rom 2 l Nov em ber1975,V o l u me1 9 0 ,p p ' 8 0 4 -8 0 6 SCIEI\UCE Host Tree LocationBehaviorof a TropicalVine ( M onstersgigontea) by S kototropism Donald R. Strong, Jr. and Thomas S. Ray, Jr. Copyrighta fi75 by the American Association lor the Aduancement of Science Host Tree Location Behaviorof a Tropical Vine ( M onstera gigantea)by Skototropism Abstract. Seedlings of the arboreal, ground-germinating, tropical v,r?eMonstera gigantea (Engler) are shown to grow directly toward potential host trees; they do not find hosts by haphazard growth or random searching. Our experimentsshow that these vines are attracted to the darkest sector of the horizon. In nature treesprovide thesedark sectors. We term this response skototropism (growth toward darkness). Skototropism is probably produced by a modifcation of the molecular and cellular mechanisms that produce negativephototopism. Vle intoduce the new term to emphasize the adaptive nature of the response; whereas the term "negative phototropism" can imply either growth away from light or growth toward darkness, only growth toward darkness can lead the vine directly to a host. This is because,in nature, hosts will not be aligned l8U from the lightest sector of the horizon relative to the vine. Arboreal vines are a mainly tropical phenomenon; species diversity of this group is much higher at low latitudes (1) where perennially warm and moist conditions allow the high surface-to-volumeratio of their elongate form. This form allows vines the unique advantage of lateral movement acrossthe canopy, to occupy diverse areas not available to trees anchored F ig . l. Se e d lin g s o f gigantea Monstera converging upon a willbe host tree, at Finca La Selva, Costa Rica. Notice erect shoot tipg which serve to constantly apprise of host location relative to the minor diversions of forest floor debris. at a single site. The capacity for lateral canopy movement, however, costs vines their self-supportive ability and presents arboreal specieswith the problem of finding a host tree to climb. Many biologists have been concernedwith the mechanism of vine host location. Darwin interpreted his experiments with the vine Bignonia capreolata to show that hosts are found by "apheliotropism" (growth away from light) (2). Other mechanismshavebeenhypothesized through the years: negative geotropism (J), random searching(2, 4), and positive phototropism (5). In this report we give the first description of actual host location in a natural vine population and demonstratethe location mechanism to be skototropism (growth toward darkness); seedlings of Monstera gigantea locate trees by growing toward the darkness of a tree silhouette. Monstera gigantea (Araceae) is a vine of Caribbean lowland forests. lt germinates terrestrially, immediately after dispersal from inflorescencesset partway up trunks of canopy trees.Seedlingshave singleslender prostrate stems with tiny bract-like leaves at nodes. There are three distinct ontogeneticmorphologiesduring the vine's progress from ground to mature height a seedling stage of horizontal terrestrial growth, a "saucer-ieafed" stage maintained during host tree ascent,and an adult stage.In the adult morphology the species flowers repeatedly in direct sunlight and continues to climb. We observed seed set to be simultaneous,with the entire contents of an inflorescence falling to the ground en masse within several meters. This means that seedlingstend to climb the parent tree, or very nearby ones. Host finding behavior of Monstera gigantea seedlingscan be conspicuousin nature. Thousandsof bright green seedlings are seenin contrast against the brown leaf litter, creeping inward from all sidesof the tree. The seedlingsappear as short spokes of a wheel with the tree as the hub (Fig. I ). In this behavior the tip of the elongating stem simply stands up, bends,and grows toward the tree, independently of compass direction betweenthe two. The mean angle of growth relative to the host tree is calculated as d 180 derlain by cellular and molecular mechanisms similar to thosecausingthe classical negativephototropism of laboratory plant physiology ( 6, Z). _3 There is no apparenttrend for individual Monslera seedlings more distant from a potentialhost tree to havegrown more toro o oa tuously than close seedlings. Seedling U ta o o ! stems at all distancesfrom potential host trees are contorted. This contortion results from growth around and over fallen leaves c and other forest floor objects. E The relativedarknessas well as distance of objects also influencesthe propensity to attract thesevine seedlings.We set pairs of seedlingsparallel to both sides of shade cloth panels at 5-cm intervals from I to 100 cm away from the panels.We usedfive o o panels with one to five thicknesses different 50 @ of cloth. A singlethicknesspasses60 percent ofthe light. The panelswere placedon 0 391521273339 the forest floor, and faced east-west.SeedDecimeters from tree lings respondeddifferently only to the first Fig. 2. Averageangleofgrowth (Z)andstandard two panels, to 40 percent as opposed to 64 deviationof growth angle(s) amongvine seed- percent shade. Responsesto the four panlings found along a 4-m transectout from the els of shade of 64, 78, 81, and 89 percent baseof a potentialhost tree.When the average were identical. At these panels all of the directionof growth is directly toward the tree E : 0, and whenall seedlings aregrowingin the closer seedlingsturned 90" and grew tosamedirections = 0. The formulasaregivenin ward the panel within 6 days;the most disthe text. Note that the vinepopulation'saccura- tant seedlingshad turned toward the pancy (a) and precision(s) of host location de- els within 30 days. Closer seedlingsat the creaseswith increasingdistancefrom the tree. panel of 40 percent shade began to turn Thesepatternsobtainin all directionsfrom potential host trees,indicatingthat the seedlings only after l2 days, and only about halfof those within 30 cm had turned by the end areattracteddirectlyto the trees. of the 34-day experiment; there was no responseat greater than 30 cm. Over 90 perIn Fig. 2 we show how the accuracyand cent of seedlingsat the higher shadeswere precision of seedling growth angle de- growing toward the panelsat the end of the creases with distance from the tree; the experiment,whereasonly 7 of l4 surviving mean growth angle wanders from 0', and on the eastsideanci9 of l4 survivingon the the variance among seedlings in growth west side of the 40 percent darkness panel angle increases with increasing distance were doing so at termination. These exfrom the potential host tree. The same periments were done beneath dense canorelationship was observedto hold on all py, where the seedlingsoccur naturally. sidesof the tree (seeFig. 1). Seedlingsfall- Hence, the seedlingsare discriminating40 en naturally around 19 treesand in several percent light intensity differenies at the n experimental situations produced con- very low intensity(10 to 100lux) ofthe for) sin a; vergencefrom all quarters. est floor. l- l a =a rcta n That the seedlingsconvergefrom all diThe attractivenessof a tree trunk is also ) c os a, rections indicates that a function they are finding the of its diameter; trees with large i= I tree by skototropism, by growth toward diameters will orient seedlingsat greater wheren is the number of seedlingsand a; is the darkest sector of the horizon indepen- distancethan those with small trunks. The the angle of growth of individual seedlings dently of the angle between this dark sec- seedlings whose growth angles are repre(/). When a : 0, average growth among tor and the lightest sector of the horizon. sented in Fig. 2 were orienting toward a seedlings is directly toward the tree. The We introduce this new term to emphasize tree 75 cm in diameter. Although the oristandard deviation in growth angle among the adaptive nature of the response.On the entation decreasesgradually with distance, seedlingsrelativeto the host tree is s (1) forest floor potential host trees are dark a distinct drop-off of orientation appears sectorson a haphazardly chiaroscuro hori- between1.9 and 2.I m. At this distancethe zon, and direction toward a potential host silhouette comprised roughly 22" to 20" of is almost never 180" from the lightest sec- the horizon. Our experimentswith the attor, relative to the seedling.Hence, this re- tractive power of experimentally placed sponse is not growth away from the light, vertical boards of various widths (l to 30 but an adaptation that serves to lead the cm) confirms that the silhouette must comvine directly to a host tree. There is no rea- prise more than a few degreesof the horison to suspectthat skototropismis not un- zon in order to attract seedlines.The nar@ [ +{-,,"[e#). I,i,"o"'\'l'l." \-, /l I rowest boards attracted from only a few centimeters,whereas the widest ones attracted seedlingsat distancesup to 70 cm. Seedling mortality resulting from burial under falling debris,leaves,and branchesis very high, and the growth of seedlingsis very slow (lessthan l0 cm/month); therefore only those seedlingswithin a few decimeters are able to reach the host before being buried under dead leaves. As skototropism might be dangerousfor plant, we set up a dark a photosynthesizing cul-de-sacto see how far vine seedlings would grow into the dark. We placeda box with three sides and a top on the ground next to severalseedlings.The open side of the box, I m wide and l5 cm high, was perpendicular to the growth direction of the vines.After 2 months the vines had grown into the box and in a sinusoidal pattern parallel to the open end.They werecaught, oscillatingjust insidethe mouth of the box. The vines apparently switch to positive phototropism when the light grows very dim, but switch again to skototropism when the light grows brighter.This switching could be homologousto the first positive and first negative phototropic responsesfound in taxa used in laboratory plant physiology. However, Mo,nstera gi' gantea shows no second positive phototropic response.At all but minimal light levels these vines are skototropic, hence this responseis distinct from those describedby classicallaboratory plant physiology (6,7). Skototropism ceaseswhen the horizontally growing seedlingmeetsa tall, vertical surface. In our experiments, seedling growth changesto positively phototropic within I month of meetingthe tree.During this period leaf rnorphology changesinto l0 . l'1,*v*, 7, 58 I that of the second,saucer-leafedstage.The positively phototropic responseof the second stage is demonstratedby the growth of seedlingsthat encounterthe tree deep in a growth crotch betweena pair of buttresses; in this case is upward but also distinctly out war d i n t o a r e a s m o r e e x p o s e dt o s u n light. The first saucer leaves are small, about I cm in diameter, and are Iight green.During ascentthe leavesdarken;the highestsaucerleavescan be 25 cm in diameter and are often dark green and covered with epiphyllae.The translormation from the saucer-leafto adult stageoccurs when the vine emergesinto direct sunlight. We infer this from observationsof the transition positionson the host trunk from saucer-leafed to adult Monstera gigantea. When the species ascends a trunk in unshaded sunlight it assumesadult morphology at only a few meters above the ground. Also, we have not found the adult morphology beneath understory canopy. Skototropism is the only meansyet proposed that will lead a vine directly to a host. Random searching (2, 4), negative geotropism (3), positive phototropism (5), or growth away from the light (2, 8) can only lead the seedlinginto an area where there may be a host, or into a light area until it where the vine can photosynthesize finds a host by random movement. Becauseof its obvious adaptivevalue skototropism may be a general mechanismfor host location in ground-germinatingarboreal vines (9). We can find only one previous author, though, with data that can be interpretedto indicate a whole-plant skototropic response.In this case an unidentified vine speciesis describedas pursuing a stake that was moved daily to a different PS,loqY, A 6,tazs position relative to the sun's diurnal course.However the author offersthis, not as evidence of skototropism, but as of plants," "proof " of the "consciousness "that they think," and that "plants belong to the philosopherclass" (10). D o N n r - o R . Sr n o N c, Jn . T s o u n s S. R ,q v,Jn . Biological Science Department, Florida State U niversity, Tallahassee 32306 Referencesand Notes l. E. Batshelet.Statistical Methods for the Analvsis of Problems in Animal Orientation and Certain Biologicat Rhvthms (American lnstitute of Biol oqi c i l Sc i enc e,w as hi ngton. D .C .' 1 9 6 5 ). 2 C. R. Darwin. rlre Movements and Habits ol Climbing Plqnts (Mvruy, London. I875). J. E. A. Menninger. f lowering l/ines of the World: An Encvclopedia o[ Climbing P/artt (Hcarthside. N ew Yor k . 1970) . 4. A. C. Hottes, A Little Book oJ Climbing Plants ( D e La M ar e. N ew Yor k , l 9l l ) . 5. b. H . J anz en, " Vi ne popul ati on d y n a mi c s a n d c om m unr l y s tr uc tur e. O r gani z ati o n f o r Tro p i e u l Studies course report," in Advanced Eiolog.u Ecolo7y in the Tropic.s(Organization for Tropical Studi e;. U ni v er s i tv of C os ta R i c a. S a n J o s e . 1 9 6 9 ) 6. A. W. Galston, iir Encyclopedia of Plant Ph,-si' ol os y , W . R uhl and. Ed. ( Spr i nge r V e rl a g . B e rl i n . f95C ) ,v ol . l ?. N o. l . pp.492 529. 7 H E Streer and H. Opif. The Phvsiologv ol Flowering P/anr.r(American Elsevier, New York, r970). 8. T . A. Kni eht, " Sec hs pfl anz enp h y s i o l o g i s c haeb handl uns en.' i n O s tw ai d's , Kl as si k e r d e r E ra k t e n W i s s enc - haJ ten. N . R .62 ( Lr epz i g. 1 8 9 5 t 9. Si nc e thi s r epor t w as s ubm i tted w e h a v e g a t h c rc d s i m i l ar ev i denc e that another C o s t a R i c a n v i n e species [Syzgoniaz, new species(Birdsey. nomen in edit)l is skototropic. This second species,howev er . hi s a di fl er eni l i fe hi s tor y a n d mo rp h o J o g l s tr r teg) . l t * i l l be des c r i bedr n s ub s e q u e npt u b l rc a ti ons ( T . S. R ay , J r ., i n pr epar atr o n ). 10. A. Bierce. "Moxon's master," in Ten Tales (Cur' w en, London, 1925) . I L W e i hank C . H ar r i s . M . E. T hor n t o n ' R D re s s l e r, R . F ox , P. H om ann, M . Bi r ds ey ,a n d C . V e z a t -R o oi c al for hel p w i th v ar r ous por ti o n s o f t h i s w o rk ' T h" doto w er e eather ed at i he La S e l v a l i e l d u n rt of the Organiz-ation for Tropical Studies' Saraoi oui . C os i a R i c a, dur i ng J une th ro u g h N o v c mb e r 19i 4. Suppor t w as fr om N Sh gra n t G B 4 2 5 1 5 ro D .R .S. 23 May 1975
© Copyright 2025 Paperzz