Sleep, 18(1):30--38 © 1995 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society Sleep Deprivation: Effects on Work Capacity, Self-Paced Walking, Contractile Properties and Perceived Exertion *c. D. Rodgers, tD. H. Paterson, t:j:D. A. Cunningham, tEo G. Noble, tF. P. Pettigrew, §W. S. Myles and t:j:A. W. Taylor *School of Physical and Health Education, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada; tFaculty of Kinesiology and +.Department of Physiology, The University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada; and §Defense and Civil Institute of Environmental Medicine Toronto, Ontario, Canada Summary: The primary purpose of this study was to examine the effect of a 48-hour period of sleep deprivation on the performance of selected physical work tasks [30-45% of maximum oxygen consumption (V0 2 max)]. In addition, this study assessed the effect of continual performance of physical work during sleep deprivation on standardized physiological and psychological test scores. Nineteen male subjects performed six different physical tasks, designed to involve all major muscle groups, during a 48-hour period of sleep deprivation. Fourteen subjects served as sleep-deprivation controls. Performance on all physical work tasks decreased significantly. Neither sleep deprivation (SD) or sleep deprivation in conjunction with continuous physical work (SDW) had any effect on muscle contractile properties, anaerobic power measures or resting blood glucose and lactate concentrations. Only SD subjects demonstrated a decline in cardiorespiratory function. Self-selected walking pace decreased and perceived exertion increased significantly in the SDW group. Positive and negative mood scores were adversely affected in both groups, the total change being greatest in SD subjects. The results indicate that performance of physical work tasks requiring 30-45% V0 2 max declines significantly over a 48-hour period of sleep deprivation. However, maximal physiological function is not unduly compromised by either the work tasks in conjunction with sleep deprivation or by sleep deprivation alone. Key Words: Sleep deprivation-Work capacity-Muscle contraction-Self-paced walking-Perceived exertion-Mood-Physicallabor. Previous studies that have examined the effect of sleep deprivation on functional task performance have focused primarily on cognitive tasks or tasks demanding a high degree of visual effort (1). In instances where the effects of sleep deprivation on the ability to sustain physical tasks have been examined, the results are dependent upon the nature of the "tasks" evaluated and the duration of the sleep-deprived period. Most often the physical tasks have been either a series of physiological measurement tests (2), interspersed low-intensity [25-30% maximum oxygen consumption (V02max)] treadmill exercise periods (3,4), or military performance tasks, the intensity of which has not been well documented (5). To date, Accepted for publication August 1994. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. C. D. Rodgers, School of Physical and Health Education, The University ofToronto, 320 Huron Street, Toronto, Ontario M5S IAI Canada. 30 there are no studies that have examined the effect of 48 hours of sleep deprivation on the performance of physical work tasks requiring an intensity of work effort equivalent to 35-40% V0 2max. This intensity is important in that it has been shown to be equivalent to the self-paced intensity of industrial workers over an 8-hour work day (6,7). Furthermore, it is believed to represent an intensity of effort that is able to be sustained up to 24 hours (8). However, no studies exist that have ascertained whether or not this functional level of activity can be maintained under sleep-deprived conditions. Hence, the first objective of this study was to examine the effects of sleep deprivation on performance of physical tasks requiring a work effort equivalent to 35-40% V0 2max. A secondary objective of this study was to examine the effect of sleep deprivation on physical work task performance in conjunction with regular evaluation of physiological function on standardized tests. In a study 31 SLEEP DEPRIVATION AND WORK CAPACITY by Haslam (5) both military exercises and performance on various physical fitness tests during sleep deprivation were examined. However, the physical fitness tests utilized were not clearly discussed, and it is difficult to determine the effects of sleep deprivation on both the functional tasks and physiological parameters. This type of evaluation has particular implications for situations such as military operations, which might require continuous physical effort in a sleep-deprived state as well as under the threat of potential emergency efforts. If performance of maximal physiological function is unduly compromised, the consequences are of major significance. METHODS SO subjects read or watched television in a lounge. In addition, every 2 hours SD subjects went for a short (less than 100 m) walk. The only interruptions to either groups' activities were meals, which were served approximately every 6 hours (at least 2 hours before testing), and repeated physiological assessment of some parameters at 12, 36 and 48 hours. A supervisor was assigned to each group at all times to ensure continued wakefulness of the subjects. Total kilocalories consumed (C = 8, 158 ± 1,150; E = 8,237 ± 1,283) across 2 days and the percentage of proteins (15 ± 3%), carbohydrates (50 ± 7%) and fat (35 ± 6%) distributed across the diet (six meals and two snacks) did not differ between the groups. Subjects were not allowed to consume any caffeine-containing substances, alcohol or drugs during the experimental period. Subjects Thirty-three male volunteers were randomly subdivided into two groups: sleep deprived + work tasks (SOW: n = 19; 23.0 ± 3.2 years; 76.5 ± 7.6 kg) and sleep deprived only (SD: n = 14; 22.6 ± 2.0 years; 77.6 ± 8.9 kg). During a 48-hour period of sleep deprivation subjects in the SDW group performed a continuous series of work tasks, whereas SO subjects read or watched TV. Both groups were tested on a variety of physiological parameters prior to, during, and upon completion of the 48-hour experimental period. All testing procedures conformed to, and the subjects were made aware of, the Declaration of Helsinki statement for the testing of human subjects. Testing procedures Phase I-familiarization One week prior to the onset of the experimental period, subjects in both groups were given a briefphysical examination and were familiarized with the Wingate test (9) and muscle contractile properties measurement protocol. The medical examination was done to ensure that all subjects were in good health and would not be unduly compromised by 48 hours of sleep deprivation. Familiarization with the Wingate test and contractile properties protocol was necessary to prevent contamination of the actual experimental data due to lack of familiarity with the testing procedures. Phase II -experimental period General procedures. A 12-hour fasting blood sample was drawn from each subject upon their arrival (7:00 a.m.) at the laboratory (Time 0). Subjects were then assessed on a variety of physiological and psychological parameters. Upon completion of this baseline test, the SOW group began a repeated series of work tasks and Physiological parameters - measurement techniques The following series of assessment tests were administered to all subjects at the onset ofthe experiment (Time 0) and upon completion of the 48-hour sleep deprivation period. The self-paced walking test was also repeated at 8 and 32 hours. 1. Blood glucose and lactate levels Blood samples were drawn from the antecubital vein and analyzed for plasma glucose and lactate levels on a Model 23 L Lactate/Glucose Analyzer (Yellow Springs Instruments). Hematocrit and hemoglobin levels were determined using standard micromethods as described by Bauer (10). 2. Muscle contractile properties Contractile properties of the triceps surae group were determined according to the methods of Klein et al. (11) using an apparatus similar to that which has been described previously by Davis and White (12). Briefly, the subject's left knee and foot were placed in a steelbar restraining apparatus, the knee being held in place by a metal plate connected to a strain gauge. A surface electrode was placed over the belly of the gastrocnemius muscle for surface electrical stimulation of the triceps surae muscle group. Muscle contractile properties were assessed at the level of twitch contraction, and at 10- , 20- and 50-Hz contractions. The contractile force signal obtained from the strain gauge was amplified and fed into a DEC A/D converter, digitalized and stored for later analysis. A measure of maximal voluntary contraction of the triceps surae group was also obtained on each subject upon completion of the contractile properties test. Sleep, Vol. 18, No.1, 1995 32 C. D. RODGERS ET AL. 3. Anaerobic peak power and average power output The Wingate test of anaerobic power (9) was used to assess anaerobic peak power (over 5 seconds) and average power output (over 30 seconds). This was done by having the subject pedal all-out for 30 seconds against a predetermined load setting [0.075 x body wt (kg)] on a Monark bicycle ergometer. From the data input of pedal revolutions per unit time and resistance, values for total work, peak, mean and low-power outputs were calculated. A fatigue index was also calculated by subtracting the low-power output from the high-power and dividing the resulting value by the high-power output. 4. Submaximal work capacity The PWC 170 test was administered to determine the submaximal work capacity of each subject. Subjects began pedaling (50-60 rpm) an electrically braked cycle ergometer at 150 W. The workload was then increased 25 W every 2 minutes until a heart rate of 170 beats/ minute was achieved. At this time the test was terminated and the final workload attained recorded. 5. Self-paced walking A self-paced walking test was administered to each subject to evaluate the effect of sleep deprivation on self-selected speeds of walking. The protocol selected was a modification of the tests ofBassey et al. (13) and Ekblom et al. (14) as has previously been described by Cunningham et al. (15). Briefly, each subject was asked to walk two lengths of an outdoor course of 30 m, in response to each of the following pace instructions: a) walk rather slowly (slow pace); b) walk at a normal pace, neither fast nor slow (normal pace); and c) walk rather fast but without overexerting yourself (fast pace). For each of these self-selected paces, velocity and pace frequency were determined. The measure of velocity has been shown to be reliable and reproducible (15), with a correlation coefficient of 0.81 and no significant differences between repeated tests. 6. Perceived exertion test To determine each subject's perception of work effort, two cycle ergometer protocols were administered. The first, the response protocol (R), necessitated that each subject pedal an electrically braked cycle ergometer (Collins Pedal mate) at 70 rpm for seven 45-second exercise bouts at loads ranging from 100 to 180 W, each separated by 15 seconds of rest. Immediately after each work bout the subject was asked to rate their perceived intensity of work effort according to the Borg scale. For the production protocol (P) the subject once again performed seven 45-second exercise bouts, sepSleep. Vol. 18. No.1. 1995 arated by 15 seconds of rest. However, in this instance the subjects themselves were responsible for setting appropriate work settings to yield an exercise intensity corresponding to a Borg scale rating of either fairly light (a rating of 11), somewhat hard (a rating of 13) or hard (a rating of 15). The order of perceived intensity for the seven workbouts was 13, 15, 11, 13, 11, 15 and 13. During both protocols subjects were prevented from observing the control panel work-load reading. A rest period of 1 minute was given between the two test protocols. Test score was calculated by averaging either the perceived rating (R protocol) or the exercise work load (P protocol) of exercise bouts 2 through 6. Psychological parameters - measurement techniques At 0, 8, 32 and 48 hours, questionnaires were administered to the subjects to assess their mood and level of fatigue (16). The mood scale contained 20 positive and nine negative words that could be used to describe one's mood (e.g. alert, jittery, happy, grouchy, etc.). Subjects were asked to score each word using a Lichert scale (not at all-O points; a little-l point; quite a bit-2 points; extremely-3 points). The higher the score on the positive words, the more positive the mood of the subjects; the higher the score on the negative words, the more negative the mood. For the assessment of fatigue, subjects rated a series of 10 statements, ranging from "very lively" to "ready to drop" on a better than (2 points), same as (1 point), or worse than (0 points) scale. Scores were totaled with low scores being indicative of a more fatigued state. Work Tasks Each subject in the SDW group performed the following series of six tasks approximately six times during the 48-hour sleep-deprivation period. The order of tasks was randomized across subjects. Breaks were only given between items to allow the subjects to walk to the next work task station, for meals or for physiological/psychological testing (8, 12 and 32 hours). All tasks were designed to provide demands similar to tasks of military field operations. The individual task protocols were as follows. Task 1: the number of sandbags carried over a distance of 5 meters in 30 minutes. Task 2: the distance walked in 30 minutes with a 12-kg packsack. Task 3: the work rate on a cycle ergometer was set at 80% of V0 2 max (predicted from submaximal exercise) for a 2-minute period. This task was repeated three times with a 10minute rest interval between each repeat. The measure used was the completion of a set work load. Task 4: the number of stakes planted in 45 minutes. Task 5 the number of revolutions of an arm ergometer wheel 33 SLEEP DEPRIVATION AND WORK CAPACITY TABLE 1. Sleep-deprived work task performance 24-48 hours 0-24 hours SDW work tasksa 18.5 ± 5.4* Task I (n = 6): Sandbags carried/30 minutes 32.5 ± 11.1 1,260 ± 480 m * 1,830 ± 810 m Task 2 (n = 9): Distance walked/30 minutes Completed Task 3: Completion-set bicycle work load Completed 29.7 ± 14.5* 43.7±9.1 Task 4 (n = 7): Stakes planted/45 minutes 995 ± 96* 1,207 ± 138 Task 5 (n = 8): Arm ergometer revolutions/2 x 15 minutes 462.5 ± 136.2 m* 812.5 ± 277.5 m Task (n = 9): Wheelbarrow loads x 15 m/30 minutes Values are represented as means ± standard deviation. a Variation in number of subjects completing different tasks is due to incomplete rotation of all subjects through all six tasks in each time segment. * Indicates a significant (p < 0.05) difference in performance between the first 24 hours and the last 24 hours. (load equivalent to 40% of the subject's V0 2 max) that could be generated in 15 minutes. Task 6: the number of times a wheelbarrow was loaded with top soil and pushed 15 m in a 30-minute period. Items were scored by counting the number of tasks completed within the defined time frame. Statistics Paired t tests were used to compare performance scores on the work tasks. Scores on the psychological and physiological performance tests were compared using analysis of variance (time, group) and analysis of covariance (self-paced walking test) techniques (BMDP program #P2V). When indicated, post hoc between-group comparisons were assessed using the Bonferroni procedure (BMDP program #P7D). perimental period. There was no significant effect of work and/or sleep deprivation on measurements of anaerobic power as determined by the Wingate test (Table 4). A significant (p < 0.05) decrease in cycle ergometer work rate at 170 beats/minute (PWC 170) was noted only in the SD group (Table 4). Figure 1 displays a synopsis of the percentage change in each of the physiological parameters for the two groups. Although there were no significant betweengroup differences, the SDW group did demonstrate a greater negative (undesirable) change in all but two (MVC; submaximal work capacity) of the 14 parameters assessed. The total percentage change across all the physiological parameters (excluding blood parameters) was -38.78% (range -10.99 to 1.57%) in the SD group and -48.6% (range -8.63 to -0.36%) in the SDW group. Physiological and psychological measurements of perception RESULTS Work tasks Over the final 24 hours of sleep deprivation, performance scores on all but the cycle ergometer task (task 3) decreased significantly (p < 0.05) (Table 1). Physiological parameters Forty-eight hours of sleep deprivation had no significant effect on blood glucose or blood lactate concentration (Table 2) in either the SD or SDW group. Muscle contractile properties of the triceps surae group (Table 3) remained relatively constant across all frequencies examined for both groups throughout the ex- Although both SD and SDW showed a decrease in self-selected walking pace at all speeds (slow, normal and fast), after 48 hours of sleep deprivation this decline was only significant (p < 0.05) in the SDW group. There was no significant difference between the two groups in the number of steps necessary to cover the 30-meter distance (Table 5). The ratings of an imposed work load (R protocol) or the production of a work load corresponding to a Borg scale rating (P protocol) did not change significantly after 48 hours of sleep deprivation in the SD group. In contrast, SDW subjects showed a significant (p < 0.05) increase in perception of work (R protocol) TABLE 2. Resting blood glucose and lactate concentrations for sleep-deprived and sleep-deprived work task groups SD group (n = 14) Parameter o hours 48 hours 4.30 ± 0.30 4.18 ± 0.48 Glucose nM/1 0.54 ± 0.29 0.58 ± 0.57 Lactate mM/1 Values are represented as means ± standard deviation. SD = sleep deprived only; SDW = sleep deprived + work tasks. SDW group (n = 19) o hours 48 hours 4.85 ± 1.00 0.65 ± 0.52 4.47 ± 0.32 0.42 ± 0.34 Sleep. Vol. 18, No.1. 1995 C. D. RODGERS ET AL. 34 TABLE 3. Contractile properties of the triceps surae for sleep-deprived and sleep-deprived work task groups SD group (n o hours = SDW group (n 14) o hours 48 hours = 19) 48 hours 8.9 ± 2.4 8.5 8.7 ± 3.0 8.5 ± 2.9 Pt (Newtons) 46.7 ± 13.1 44.1 42.6 ± 14.5 43.8 ± 15.7 Po 10 Hz (Newtons) 75.3 ± 20.9 70.3 ± 23.1 68.8 67.0 ± 21.6 Po 20 Hz (Newtons) 97.6 ± 29.6 92.3 91.9 ± 28.0 87.1 ± 26.8 Po 50 Hz (Newtons) 136.6 ± 25.9 128.1 137.1 ± 39.4 125.2 ± 34.3 MVC (Newtons) Values are represented as means ± standard deviation. Pt = twitch tension; Po = tension at Hz indicated; MVC = maximal voluntary contraction; SD = sleep deprived only; deprived + work tasks. and a significant (p < 0.05) decrease in performance (P protocol) (Table 6). As was to be expected, both groups demonstrated a significant (p < 0.05) decrease (indicative of being more fatigued) in their fatigue rating after 48 hours of sleep deprivation (Table 6). SDW subjects demonstrated a significant (p < 0.05) deterioration in their mood by 32 hours (Table 7). This was in contrast to SD subjects who did not show a significant (p < 0.05) decline in mood until 48 hours. A summary of the percentage change in each of the physiological/psychological measurements of perception is displayed in Fig. 2. The SDW group demonstrated a greater negative (undesirable) change in all but one (negative psychological score) of the nine parameters assessed. Total percentage change across measurements of perception was -426.3 in the SD group (range - 281.3 to - 6.6) and - 396.0 in the SDW group (range - 215 to - 7.7). DISCUSSION Of critical importance in this study was the finding that performance on all work tasks decreased significantly after 48 hours of sleep deprivation. Moreover, this decrease in performance occurred despite a continued ability to maintain performance on physiological test items reflecting functional capacity. This would suggest that although sleep-deprived individuals may have the physiological capacity to do the work, the interference of mood, perception of effort or even the ± 2.5 ± ± ± ± 13.5 20.9 26.5 22.2 SDW = sleep repetitive nature of the tasks decreases the ability of individuals to maintain a constant level of work output in a sleep-deprived state. This speculation is further supported by the decrease in performance in the SDW condition only on all tasks involving the perception of effort. The primary implication of these findings is that continuous physical tasks at 35-40% V0 2 max are inhibited by 48 hours of sleep deprivation but maximal efforts can still be achieved. Thus, in situations requiring an all-out effort, it appears that the necessary level of physiological function could still be achieved. The primary purpose of this study was to determine the effect of a 48-hour period of sleep deprivation on the performance of physical work tasks requiring a work intensity of 35-40% V0 2 max. The observed decrement in physical work task performance suggests that such tasks are unable to be continued at optimal levels of performance during 48 hours of sleep deprivation. This finding is particularly important because most industrial environments require a self-paced effort of this intensity over a typical work day (6,7). Although Home and Pettitt (8) have indicated that this intensity of effort can be sustained up to 24 hours, results from the present study indicate that by 48 hours of continued wakefulness significant performance declines occur. Previous work by Haslam (5) found that 48-72 hours of continued wakefulness is likely to render soldiers militarily ineffective. Although the intensity of the work effort by the soldiers in the Haslam study was not documented, many of the tasks performed (shooting, weapon-handling, digging, marching and patrolling) TABLE 4. Peak power, mean power, fatigue index and PWC170 workrate for sleep-deprived and sleep-deprived + work task groups SD group (n Parameter Peak power output (W·kg-) Mean power output (W·kg-') Fatigue index (%) PWC 170 (watts at 170 beats/minute) o hours = 14) 48 hours SDW group (n = 19) o hours 12.0 ± 1.8 11.7 ± 2.0 11.9±1.4 9.3 ± 0.9 9.5 ± 1.0 9.0 ± 1.6 44.5 ± 8.2 45.2 ± 7.8 40.3 ± 9.0 273 ± 41.0 243 ± 35.0* 276 ± 50 Values are represented as means ± standard deviation. SD = sleep deprived only; SDW = sleep deprived + work tasks. * Indicates a significant (p < 0.05) difference in performance after 48 hours of sleep deprivation. Sleep, Vol. 18, No. I, 1995 48 hours 10.9 8.9 39.1 275 ± 2.2 ± 1.2 ± 12.8 ± 79 SLEEP DEPRIVATION AND WORK CAPACITY 35 TABLE 5. Self-paced walking speed andfrequency (30-m course) for sleep-deprived and sleep-deprived + work task groups SD group (n = 14) = 19) SDW group (n o hours 48 hours o hours 48 hours Slow Speed (m/second) Frequency (steps/minute) 1.60 ± 0.58 118 ± 19 1.37 ± 0.47 111 ± 16 1.14 ± 0.24 96 ± 13 0.93 ± 0.18* 79 ± 11 Medium Speed (m/second) Frequency (steps/minute) 2.30 ± 0.77 145 ± 12 2.02 ± 0.66 136 ± 16 1.60 ± 0.35 108±8 1.31 ± 0.24* 96 ± 8 Fast Speed (m/second) Frequency (steps/minute) 3.13 ± 1.21 165 ± 8 2.66 ± 1.02 152 ± 9 2.14 ± 0.53 125 ± 8 1.78 ± 0.32* 113 ± 10 Pace Parameter Values are represented as means ± standard deviation. SD = sleep deprived only; SDW = sleep deprived + work tasks. * Indicates a significant (p < 0.05) difference in performance after 48 hours of sleep deprivation and physical activity. were similar in nature to those used in the present study. In contrast, other studies have demonstrated that physical work can be maintained over similar or longer periods of sleep deprivation. However, the intensity and/or duration of the physical work tasks assessed in these studies was lower than that required by the subjects in the present study. This would suggest that either the required work intensity of the tasks in the present study was too high, or that the duration of work time was too long. A more frequent analysis of performance data, perhaps on l2-hour intervals, might have provided a clearer picture of the duration over which this intensity of effort can be maintained. Evaluation of task performance of both the SD and SDW groups at time 0 and at 48 hours might also have provided a clearer picture of the role of self-pacing in continued task performance. The addition of a non-sleep-deprived control group would also help to isolate more clearly the effects of the sleep-deprived condition from the potential confounding effects of the repetitive nature and/or inherent boredom of the tasks on task performance. It is interesting to note that the greatest decrements in performance occurred in those tasks requiring significant involvement of both the upper and lower body. The number of sandbags carried in 30 minutes and the distance traveled with a loaded wheelbarrow showed a mean loss of 55% and 40%, respectively, after 48 hours of sleep deprivation. In contrast, pedal rate on the bicycle ergometer was maintained, whereas the number of revolutions on the arm ergometer and the distance walked during 30 minutes decreased by only 17% and 23%, respectively. This variation in response would suggest that the exact nature of the task that is to be performed over a prolonged period of time plays a significant role in the degree of decrement experienced. Although physiological function (as reflected by the lack of difference in the individual parameters measured in this study) was not unduly compromised, the total percentage decline in physiological function experienced by the SDW subjects was greater than that observed in the SD subjects. It is possible that the effect of this combined total body decrement is, in part, reflected in the greater percentage change in the performance of these "total" body physical activities. However, the importance of the role of psychological factors and the need for self-pacing in the observed task performance decrement can also not be overlooked. The second objective of this study was to examine the effect of the continuous performance of physical work tasks on various psychological and physiological parameters. It is quite common in military situations to require maximal efforts after long-term periods of sleep deprivation and physical work; however, the impact of this combined condition has received limited attention. In general, as assessed by the individual standardized tests, there was no significant effect of physical work during sleep deprivation on physiological functional capacity. The SDW group did demonstrate, however, a greater total decline in physiological capability over the 48-hour sleep-deprivation period. This TABLE 6. Perceptual task scores for sleep-deprived and sleep-deprived + work task groups SD group (n = 14) Parameter RPE response protocola Workrate production (watts) Fatigue ratings o hours 9.9 ± 211 ± 13.1 ± (n= 1.1 22 2.7 7) SDW group (n = 19) 48 hours o hours 48 hours 10.6 ± 1.3 197 ± 31 6.0 ± 3.3* (n = 7) 10.4 ± 0.8 197 ± 39 13.8 ± 2.8 11.2 ± 1.1* 175 ± 39* 5.3 ± 1.8* (n = 11) (n = 11) Values are represented as means ± standard deviation. SD = sleep deprived only; SDW = sleep deprived + work tasks; RPE = . a Increase in score indicates more "difficult". * Indicates a significant (p < 0.05) difference in performance after 48 hours of sleep deprivation. Sleep. Vol. 18. No. I. 1995 C. D. RODGERS ET AL. 36 Perception Related Parameters 40~---------------------------------------. ""5 en ~ 0.. en 30 ~ u. ~ " ~ 0.. en en a: z 0.. 0.. "8.9 e 0.. 0.. Q) ::> .2' 'iii u. ~ ~ & Q) > :a 5l' Z 20 Q) -10 C> c: ~ ·150 o #- -20 ·200 -30 -~~--------------------------------------~ a ~ ~ u:: 0:: .D .D ~ a0..'" a 0.. ::;: ~ ~ ::;: en '0 0.. 0.. Physiological Parameters FIG. 1. Percentage change in performance on physiological tests following 48 hours of sleep deprivation. MPO/kg bw = mean power output (Wingate test); PPO/kg bw = peak power output (Wingate test); PI = fatigue index (Wingate test); SMWC = submaximal work capacity; Pt = twitch tension; PolO = tension at 10 Hz; Po20 = tensio"n at 20 Hz; Po50 = tension at 50 Hz; MVC = maximal voluntary contraction. FIG. 2. Percentage change in performance on perceptual and psychological tests following 48 hours of sleep deprivation. SPW-S = self-paced walk: slow speed; SPW-N = self-paced walk: normal speed; SPW-F = self-paced walk: fast speed; Positive = positive mood score test; Negative = negative mood score test. In the present study, both SD and SDW subjects showed no significant decline in performance in either muscle finding is similar to the work of others but expands contractile ability or anaerobic power output. Thus the the physiological parameters examined to include mus- addition of physical work tasks during the sleep-decle contractile properties and self-paced walking ef- prived period indicated no further physiological impairment to the SDW subjects, and when maximal forts. Symons et al. (2) demonstrated that after 60 hours efforts were required, both groups could perform. An of sleep deprivation neither anaerobic ability nor max- analysis of the low-frequency tetanic tension data does imal isometric and isokinetic strength were impaired. suggest, however, that SDW subjects may have experienced a degree of low-frequency muscular fatigue as TABLE 7• Mood scale ratings for sleep-deprived and sleep- a result of their physical task performance. Although the decrease in tension for isometric tetanic contracdeprived + work task groups tions obtained through electrical stimulation was not Parameter SD(n= 14) SDW (n = IS) statistically different between the two groups, the dePositive scalea crease was twice as great in the SDW group. This type o hours 37.3 ± 6.1 35.7 ± 7.8 of low-frequency fatigue following muscular contrac8 hours 35.3 ± 7.9 34.9 ± 6.3 tion has been demonstrated in several earlier studies 32 hours 30.1 ± 10.1 24.2 ± 8.0* 48 hours 23.0 ± 11.4* 19.7 ± 7.7* (for review see reference 17) and, when taken in conjunction with a lack of change in anaerobic power outNegative scaleb o hours 3.2 ± 3.3 4.0 ± 2.0 put, suggests that the high-threshold or fast-twitch mo8 hours 3.3 ± 3.1 4.3 ± 2.8 tor units were not fatigued by the added physical tasks, 32 hours 7.8 ± 4.9 11.4 ± 6.4* whereas low-frequency (slow twitch) motor units did 48 hours 12.2 ± 5.8* 12.6 ± 3.9* tend to exhibit signs of early impairment. Such a specValues are represented as means ± standard deviation. ulation might also explain the greater percentage decSD = sleep deprived only; SDW = sleep deprived + work tasks. a Decrease in score = less positive mood. rement observed in performance in those physical tasks b Increase in score = more negative mood. that required a more "total body" effort. * Indicates a significant (p < 0.05) difference in mood rating when In the present study, the effect of sleep deprivation compared to 0 hours. Sleep, Vol. 18, No.1, 1995 37 SLEEP DEPRIVATION AND WORK CAPACITY and sleep deprivation plus work on cardiorespiratory function was examined by evaluating each subject's work capacity at a heart rate of 170 beats/minute. It was of interest to observe in this study that only the SD subjects showed a loss of cardiorespiratory function over the period of sleep loss, as demonstrated by their decrease in work rate at this heart rate. SDW subjects were able to maintain their initial level of work capacity, and thus their initial level of cardiorespiratory performance. Previous studies have shown V0 2 max to be unchanged (18-21) or to be decreased (3,22) following periods of sleep loss. The decline in V0 2 max most often has been attributed to an increased plasma volume and associated decrease in O 2 carrying capacity of the blood (22,23) and/or to a decrease in the oxygen cost of breathing (3). Recent work by Goodman et al. (23) has extended the potential role of plasma volume despite the conflicting V0 2 max data and has suggested that the variable effects of sleep deprivation on cardiorespiratory performance may in fact reflect the conflicting influence of the positive features of enhanced plasma volume, such as increased preload in conjunction with a decrease in blood viscosity, and the negative features of a decrease in hemoglobin. In light of the discrepancy in results observed in the present study between the SDW and SD groups, it is possible that performance of the physical work tasks by the SDW subjects enabled the positive influences of enhanced blood volume to outweigh the negative effects, thus enabling a greater level of cardiorespiratory performance to be maintained. The final group of parameters assessed in this study were indices related to perception of effort and measures of mood and fatigue. Results of the self-paced walking test, R protocol and P protocol indicated that only the SDW group experienced a significant change in their perception of work effort over the course of the study. This would suggest that it is not the fatigue elicited by sleep deprivation that accounts for this change in perception, but, more likely, the fatigue associated with physical work in conjunction with sleep deprivation. In a series of experiments using a combination ofRPE assessment over prolonged work bouts (50 minutes) and somewhat similar Rand P protocols, Myles (24) showed that the duration of the exercise bout and the physical fatigue associated with the exercise bout are critical factors in determining whether a change in RPE occurs. In short-duration activities (30 seconds) sleep deprivation of 54 hours was found to have no effect on RPE, whereas physical fatigue had only a small effect. In contrast, for activities lasting more than several minutes, RPE increases progressively as a function of both physical fatigue and sleep loss. The present data would support an even greater role of physical fatigue in altering the perception of work effort in short-duration (45-second) activities. It is likely that the difference between these two studies is due to the extensive nature of the physical activity performed during the sleep-deprived period. To date, this is the first study to examine the effects of sleep deprivation and physical work on self-paced walking. The lack of change in self-paced walking speed in the SD group in this study supports earlier findings of Soule and Goldman (25), who failed to show any change in self-paced treadmill walking with a 150-30kg backpack after 31 hours of sleep deprivation, despite an increase in RPE. However, the significant change in walking speed at all paces in the SDW group emphasizes once again the role of the performance of physical tasks during the sleep-deprived period in altering the subject's perception of effort. Although there was a more rapid decline in mood in the SDW group, the overall percentage decline (positive + negative mood rating) was greatest in the SD subjects (SD = 317; SDW = 262.8). This difference appears to be primarily due to the greater increment in the negative mood score in the SD subjects. Exercise previously has been identified as a non-task-situation factor that might enhance arousal during sleep deprivation (26). The present data suggest that exercise, although not improving overall mood state, might serve to offset negative mood changes. In summary, the data from the present study indicate that although the performance of physical work tasks requiring an effort equivalent to 35-40% V0 2 max declines significantly over a 48-hour period of sleep deprivation, maximal physiological function is not unduly compromised by either the work tasks in conjunction with sleep deprivation or by sleep deprivation alone. Individuals who do participate in physical activity while sleep deprived are more likely to experience both a significant change in their perceived effort to complete certain tasks and a more rapid change in mood state. The total percentage decline in mood state does appear, however, to be offset by physical work task performance during the sleep-deprived period. Acknowledgements: This research was supported by a grant from the Defence and Civil Institute of Environmental Medicine, Downsview, Ontario. The authors thank former graduate students M. A. Babcock, P. M. MacPherson, K. M. Roberts, C. L. Rice, M. M. Sopper and R. T. Thayer for their valuable assistance in the data collection for this project. REFERENCES 1. Quant J.R. The effect of sleep deprivation and sustained military operations on near visual performance. Aviat Space Environ Med 1992;63:172-6. 2. Symons JD, VanHelder T, Myles WS. Physical performance Sleep, Vol. 18, No.1, 1995 C. D. RODGERS ET AL. 38 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. and physiological responses following 60 hours of sleep deprivation. Med Sci Sports Exerc 1988;20(4):374-80. Plyley MJ, Shephard RJ, Davis GM, Goode RC. Sleep deprivation and cardiorespiratory function: influence of intermittent submaximal exercise. Eur J Appl PhysioI1987;56:338-44. Angus RG, Heslegrave RJ, Myles WS. Effects of prolonged sleep deprivation, with and without chronic physical exercise, on mood and performance. Psychophysiology 1985;22(3):276-82. Haslam DR. The military performance of soldiers in sustained operations. Aviat Space Environ Med 1984;55(3):216-21. Astrand I. Degree of strain during building work as related to individual aerobic work capacity. Ergonomics 1967;10:293-303. Rodahl K, Vokac Z, Fugelli P, Vaage 0, Maehlum S. Circulatory strain, estimated energy output and catecholamine excretion in Norwegian coastal fishermen. Ergonomics 1974;17:585-602. Horne JA, Pettitt AN. Sleep deprivation and the physiological response to exercise under steady-state conditions in untrained subjects. Sleep 1984;7:168-79. Bar-Or 0, Dotan R, Inbar O. A 30-s all-out ergometric testits reliability and validity for anaerobic capacity. Israel J Med Sci 1977;13:126. Bauer JD. Clinical laboratory methods, 9th ed. S1. Louis: C.V. Mosby, 1982. Klein C, Cunningham DA, Paterson DH, Taylor AW. Fatigue and recovery contractile properties of young and elderly men. Eur J Appl PhysioI1988;57:684-90. Davis CTM, White MJ. Muscle weakness following dynamic exercise in humans. J Appl PhysioI1982;53:236-41. Bassey EJ, Fenten PH, MacDonald IC, Scriven PM. Self-paced walking as a method of exercise testing in elderly and young men. Clin Sci Molec Med 1976;51:609-12. Ekblom B, Day WC, Hartley LH, Moore F, Waar R. Reproducibility of exercise prescribed by pace description. Scand J Sports Sci 1979; I: 16-9. Sleep, Vol. 18, No.1, 1995 15. Cunningham DA, Rechnitzer PA, Pearce ME, Donner AP. Determinants of self-selected walking pace across ages 19-66. J GerontoI1982;37:560-4. 16. Myles WS. Sleep deprivation, physical fatigue, and the perception of exercise intensity. Med Sci Sports Exerc 1985; 17:580-4. 17. Burke RE, Edgerton VR. Motor unit properties and selective involvement in movement. In: Wilmore JH, Keogh JF, eds. Exercise and sport sciences reviews. New York: Academic Press, 1975. 18. Martin BJ. Effect of sleep deprivation on tolerance of prolonged exercise. Eur J Appl PhysioI1981;47:345-54. 19. Martin BJ, Gaddis GM. Exercise after sleep deprivation. Med Sci Sports Exerc 1981;13(40):220-3. 20. Martin BJ, Chen H. Sleep loss and the sympathoadrenal response to exercise. Med Sci Sports Exerc 1984;16(1):56-9. 21. Martin BJ, Haney R. Self-selected exercise intensity is unchanged by sleep loss. Eur J Appl PhysioI1982;9:79-86. 22. Plyley MJ, Shephard RJ, Angus AJ. Physical work and tolerance of sleep deprivation. In: Proceedings of the International Conference on Occupational Ergonomics. Ottawa: Mutual Press, 1984. 23. Goodman JM, Plyley MJ, Hart LEM, Radomski M, Shephard RJ. Moderate exercise and hemodilution during sleep deprivation. Aviat Space Environ Med 1990;61:139-44. 24. Myles WS. Sleep deprivation, physical fatigue, and the perception of exercise intensity. Med Sci Sports Exerc 1985; 17(4): 580-4. 25. Soule R, Goldman R. Pacing of intermittent work during 31 hours. Med Sci Sports Exerc 1973;5:128-31. 26. Johnston LC. Sleep deprivation and performance. In: Webb WB, ed. Biological rhythms, sleep, and performance. Chichester, England: John Wiley and Sons, 1982.
© Copyright 2025 Paperzz